Horse-Race Journalism: Reporting the Polls in the 1976 Presidential Election
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Horse-Race Journalism: Reportingthe Polls in the 1976 PresidentialElection C. ANTHONY BROH The kindsof data obtainedby publicopinionresearchand disseminated in themass mediaseemdesignedmoreto entertain thanto inform.The qualityof theinformation conveyedseemsnotmuchdifferent fromthat conveyedin thesportspagesor,betteryet,thedailyracingform. MILTON ROKEACH(1968) The polls have not been as imaginativeas they could have been in elucidatingthe real issues and underlyingforces in the election. There has been too much fixationon the horse race. -ALBERT H. CANTRIL (1976) A ND they'reoff.Comingout of the gate,the first"Polls Shows [sic] Blacks Decisive for Carter." MidwaythroughAugust,"Poll Shows Slide in CarterMargin."Into September,"Polls Show Ford Trailingin Bid forTwo VoterGroupsGOP Needs." At thehalfway markin thecampaign,"VoterPoll FindsDebate AidedFordand Cut Carter'sLead." As thecandidatesapproachthefinalmonthof cam- paigning,"First Time Ford Is Ahead of Carter,"and even more exciting,"Poll Calls Race Tied." Andin thestretch,"SurveyShows CarterHoldsLead." Atthewireit's Jimmy Carterelectedpresident. Thatis theway thelead storiesin theNew YorkTimescalled the Bicentennialrunning ofJimmy Carterand GeraldFord.The candidate maytake a shortlead out of the gate,becomefatiguedand change Abstract Reportingof public opinion polls conformedto a horse-race image of cam- paign reportingduringthe 1976 presidentialelection. Journalistsavoided prediction, reportedsegmentsof the sample, selectivelycompared results,emphasized spectacles, questioned the validityof polling, made a few mistakes, and ignoredcertain data in theirreporting.All these activitiesreinforcedthe image of elections as a sportingevent. C. AnthonyBroh is an AssistantProfessorof Political Science at Columbia Univer- sityand a Research Associate in the Instituteof Policy Sciences and Public Affairsat Duke University. Vol. 44:514-529? 1980by The Trusteesof ColumbiaUniversity PublicOpinionQuarterly Published by Elsevier North-Holland, Inc. 0033-362X/80/0044-514/$1.75 This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 515 stridesat the quarterpole, lose momentum and slow down on the back stretch, or winby a nose because of a mediablitzin thehome stretch.The race is excitingto followfrombeginning to end. Forjournalists,thehorse-racemetaphor providesa framework for analysis.A horseis judged not by its absolutespeed or skillbut in comparisonto the speed of otherhorses,and especiallyby its wins and losses. Similarly, candidatesare pushedto discussothercandi- dates;eventsare understood in a contextofcompetition; and picking thewinnerbecomesan important topic.The race-not thewinner-is the story.The candidate'simage,personality, staffrelations,and strategy are the mainfociof reporting. Furthermore, withthehorse-race metaphor journalistscan generate interest amongvoters,mostofwhomdon'tgetthechanceto observe thecandidatesin person.By reporting and interpretingeventsforthe uninvolvedelectoratethrough an easilyunderstoodimage,themass mediaforgea vitallinkin a democracybetweenthepeopleand their electedofficials. Thus the seemingtrivialization of one of America's greatestdemocraticphenomenaactuallyfostersthe democraticpro- cess. However,the imageof a horserace presentssomeproblems.Im- portantissues of publicpolicymay go unnoticedif the candidates agree on a position,and conversely,seemingly unimportant issues may receive undue attentionbecause they fit the horse-race metaphor.For example,in 1976Jimmy Carter'scontradictory utter- ances about the resignation of FBI DirectorClarenceKelly and his off-the-cuff statements in a Playboymagazineinterview aboutadul- teryreceivedgreatattention. These storieswerenotaboutissues of publicpolicy;journalistsperceivedthemin the framework of cam- paignstrategy-the conductoftherace. However,theycan affectthe outcome.The ClarenceKelly "issue" may have influenced voters who thoughtCarterwas "fuzzyon issues." Similarly, thePlayboy interview mayhave adverselyinfluenced voterswho thought of Car- ter as a moral,Christianleader. But neither"issue" informed the votersabout Carter'spublicpolicyconcerns. Even theevaluationoftheincumbent president's recordis distorted by thehorse-race metaphor. In 1976thepresspresentedGeraldFord as a candidateproneto fallsand spills,an imagesupported by a film clip of the presidentslippingon the stairsof Air Force One. When Forddescribedthepeopleof Poland,Yugoslavia,and Rumaniain the secondteleviseddebateas "freefromSovietdomination," thegaffe reinforced thephysicalimageofa stumbling, bumbling president. The statement was not consistentwithFord's two-yearrecordin office; yetthestorywas newsworthy becauseitfitthehorse-race metaphor. Thus the metaphoris both greatlybeneficialand occasionally This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
516 C. ANTHONY BROH harmful to theelectoralprocess.It providesjournalistswitha mean- ingfulframework, but thisveryframework can inducedistortion.It can heightencitizeninvolvement,butat thecostofignoring important issues. This articlefocuseson theeffectof thehorse-raceimageon jour- nalists'reporting. itexamineshowreporting Morespecifically, public opinionpolls conformedto the horse-raceframework in the 1976 presidentialcampaign. the Polls Interpreting Aboveall else,publicopinionpollsshowwhois currently ahead.A studyof 500 articlesduringthe 1976presidential election(Carmody, 1976:21)suggeststhatvirtually all articlesusingpollsreportedwhich candidateis ahead. My analysisof65 articlesin theNew YorkTimes, 57 nationaltelevisionnews storiesaboutpolls, and 8 nationalnews magazinestories(Time and Newsweek)betweenLabor Day and ElectionDay 1976 concurswith Carmody'sconclusion.The few storiesthatdid not reportwho is ahead dealt exclusivelywiththe methodology of the survey.' Withintheformat of reportingwho is ahead,journalistshave con- siderableflexibility in theirinterpretation of poll data. They can predicttheelectionoutcomeor simplyreportthefindings. Theycan reportthe percentagesupportforeach candidateor the difference betweenthe candidates.They can reportthe data for the entire electorateor for subcategoriesof it. They can select a point of comparisonwithwhichto interpret thepolls. Theycan reportvoter reactionto spectaclesduringthe campaignratherthanreportthe currentpopularity of each candidate.They can reportpoll results accuratelyor erroneously. They can questionthe validityof a par- ticularpoll. Finally,theycan ignorepolls. In the 1976 campaign, reportersmadeuse ofall thesetechniques.In mostcases, theirusage emphasizedthe horse-raceimageof the campaign.Let us examine thesepatternsof poll reporting. FORECASTING theoutcomeof a campaignmaybe themostnewswor- Forecasting thyaspectof a poll. Thus one mightexpectjournaliststo takeevery I Reportsofpollsinthe1976electiongenerally discussedthesampling error,butfew quotedtheactualwording ofthequestions(see Carmody,1976).TheNew YorkTimes and televisionnetworknews focusedexclusivelyon the methodology of surveyre- searchin 9 percentand 3 percent,respectively, of theirstorieson polls. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 517 Table 1. Referencesto PublicOpinionPolls and thePresidencyin theNew York Timesand TelevisionEveningNews fromSeptember1 to November2, 1976 New York Times TelevisionNews (N = 65) (N = 57) Forecastingof results in report ( 6%) (16%) Forecasts tie or too close to call 5 14 Forecasts winner 2 2 Figures of presidentialtrial-heatreported (15) (25) Subcategoriesof population reported (61) (56) Sample of region, state, county,or city only 31 24 Issue preferencecategoryreported 15 12 Candidate stylepreferencecategoryreported 5 8 Social groupingreported 23 10 Uncertaingroup of voters reported 14 8 Shiftsin polls reported (12) (14) Compared with pre-Labor Day 5 2 Compared withpost-Labor Day 3 2 Compared withprevious election 2 3 Compared with unspecifiedtime 6 10 Report on spectacle of campaign (17) (14) Debates 11 12 East European gaffe,Playboy Interview,etc. 3 0 Turnoutof Election Day 3 2 Distortionof poll results (5) (2) Reportedin media 3 2 Unreportedin media 2 0 Ignoringresults (6) (3) NOTE: Each entryis the percentage of articles employinga stated technique for reportingpublic opinion polls. The percentagesdo not total 100o since one articlemay have used several techniques. Numbers in italics are the net percentagesof articles in each category. opportunity to make predictions.But forecasting has its perilsfor journalists.Once theyhave forecastthe results,journalistswould have very littleleftto say; why worryabout the process if the outcomeseems clear-cut?Moreover,earlyforecastsmightnegate citizeninterest in theprocessitself,makethereporter look foolishif he is wrong,and raise the specterof journalism'sinfluencing the outcomeit has predicted.My analysisof the mediafoundonlytwo instancesof forecasting the winner,bothverylate in the campaign. One weekbeforetheelection,theNew YorkTimescarriedan article quotingpoliticalscientistGerald Pomper,who stated that Carter would win with54 percent.Two days beforeElectionDay, ABC carrieda filmclip of Jimmy Carterstatingthathe wouldwin,while theFordWhiteHouse, also reflecting on thepolls,said therace was thatdidnotuse an outsidesource too close to call. The onlyforecasts camewithinthefinaltwoweeksofthecampagin,andforecasta close race, too close to call. Thus I foundno instancesof journalists themselves forecasting thewinner.In total,theTimesreportedfore- This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
518 C. ANTHONY BROH casts of any kind in only 6 percentof the articlesabout public opinion;eveningtelevisionnews storiesdid so in 16 percent.2 REPORTING THE FIGURES Anothertechniquethatheightens interestin the horserace itself, not its winner,involvesselectiveuse of the data on presidential Of the 65 New YorkTimes articlesreferring trial-heats.3 to public opinionpolls, 15 percentreportedthe actual percentagesof each candidateinthepresidential The other85 percentpresented trial-heat. the differencebetweenthe candidates,percentages fora portionof the population,the answersto questionsotherthanvote intention, etc. Televisionnews storieswere only slightlymore directeven thoughvisual presentation of figureswithaudio overlayis equally appropriate forthismedium.Only25 percentof thetelevisionnews storiesreportedthepercentages favoring each candidatebothorally and visually. Not reporting the percentagesfor the candidateswas a policy decisionat theTimes,madein an effortto avoidthehorse-race image of the campaign.4Ironicallythe policymay have had the opposite effect.Sincejournalists couldnotreportwhowas ahead,theyfocused on otheraspectsof the poll-such as the percentagedifference be- tweenthecandidates,increases(or decreases)since an earlierpoll, or, in one case, the actual percentagesreportedby otherpolling agencies-all ofwhichemphasizethehorse-race imagebyfocusing on changesin the positionof the horseson the track. SUBCATEGORIES OF THE POPULATION Reportersmost oftenuse polls to presentthe preferences of a subcategory of the population.They use five subcategories:a re- gional,state,county,or citysample;a nationalpoll of issue prefer- ences; a nationalpoll of preferences on candidatestyle;a social grouping in the population;and uncertainvoters. Regionalpolls. Electionyearsproducemanypolls in smallgeo- 2 The differencebetweenmediaforall reporting techniqueswas not significantly X2 = .783; df = 1; p = n.s. different: 3 Trial-heat refersto thequestionthatasks howpeoplewouldvote iftheelection wereheldon thedayoftheinterview. Since 1936,theGallupOrganizationhas always asked thisquestionwithonlyslightvariations in wording.In 1976registeredvoters were asked: "If the PresidentialElectionwere beingheld today,whichcandidates wouldyouvotefor-theDemocratic candidates CarterandMondale,ortheRepublican candidatesFord and Dole?" 4 See thepaperby William Kovach in thisissue. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 519 Table 2. National and State Polls of PresidentialTrial-HeatsReportedin the New York Times and TelevisionEveningNews fromSeptember1 to November2, 1976 New York Times TelevisionNews State No directreferenceto agency 8 5 Field 3 3 BuffaloEveningNews 2 0 DardenResearch 2 0 ChicagoSun Times 1 1 ChicagoTribune 1 0 MinneapolisTribune 1 0 RutgersUniversity 1 1 Wilmington News Journal 1 0 Other 1 0 Columbus,OHa 0 1 Milwaukee,Wia 0 1 Subtotal 21 12 National New YorkTimes(CBS) 27 6 Gallup(Newsweek) 13 13 No directreference to agency 8 10 Caddell 6 1 Harris(ABC) 6 18 Roper(PBS) 4 2 Yankelovich(Time) 3 0 Other 3 10 ChiltonResearch(AssociatedPress) 2 2 NielsonRatings 2 0 NBC News 0 3 Subtotal 74 65 Grandtotal 95 77 AgencynotReported. a Polling NOTE:The networks emphasizedtheircommissioned pollsmorethanotherpolls;the ABC-commissioned HarrispollandtheNBC Newspollwerereported moreon televi- sionthanin theTimes. Conversely, theTimes referred moreoftento itsjointlyspon- soredpollwithCBS thanto anyotherpoll. graphicareas. The Field Poll, for example,has a reputationfor reliablyassessingvoteropinionin California; theRutgersPoll shares a similarreputationin New Jersey;and severalnewspaperpolls are widelyrespected.State polls generateinterestamongnonstateresi- dentsbyproviding information aboutpivotalstatessuchas California, New York,New Jersey, or Illinois.Thus,moststatepollsreported by theTimesor theTV networks wereconductedin northern industrial stateswitha history ofbeingon thewinning side. Such pollsindicate thetrendsin keystatesthatmayaffecttheoutcome(see Broh,1981). Thirty-one percentofthereferences to pollingin thenewspapers, and 24 percenton televisionwere to state,county,or citypolls. Issue preferences.Whenissue-oriented votersprefer one candidate This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
520 C. ANTHONY BROH on one issue buttheothercandidateon a secondissue,thefocuson issuesis particularly suitedto capturing voterinterest.A Times/CBS poll released on October 15, for example,showed the candidate preferencesofvoterswithvariousissuepositions.The report(Apple, 1976:B4)states:"One ofthekeyissuesis economic.The Times/CBS surveyshowedforexample,thatMr. Carteris winningoverwhelm- inglyamongthosewhociteunemployment as theirdominantconcern, as wouldbe expected." But the same poll showedthatCarterwas onlyeven withFord amongthoseconcernedaboutinflation. ThustheTimesreaderwhois worriedaboutbothunemployment andinflation receiveda messageof a closelycontestedcampaign. Accordingto a reportof theNationalCouncilon PublicPolls and theGannettJournalism Centerat Northwestern University,8 percent of the newspaperarticlesin 1976 dealt withissues in thismanner (Carmody,1976:21).Thesefindings concurroughly withmyanalysis, whichshows 15 percentof theTimesreportsand 12 percentof the televisionnewsreportsfocusing on thissubcategory. Whilenotover- whelming, thisamountofreportage on issuessuggeststhatthehorse- race metaphordoes not lend itselfto reporting on issues thatare complexor hotlybut simplistically contested. Preferences on candidate style. For example, the New York Times/CBS surveyshowedCarterleadingamongpeoplewhosaw him as a conservative(Reinhold,1976a; 1976b:32).Five percentof the New YorkTimesarticlesand 8 percentofthetelevisioneveningnews broadcastsabout public opinionpolls focusedon preferences for candidatestyle.Storiesreferred to confidencein the president, and thehonesty,credibility,and characterof bothcandidates.Whilethe characterof the candidatesis obviouslyof crucialimportance fora meaningful democraticchoice,thehorse-racemetaphor runstherisk of emphasizingbeauty-somehorsesare gorgeousanimals-and ne- glectingdifferenceson issues of substance. Social groups. People of differentreligions,occupations,races, regions,and ages have varyingcandidateand issue preferences. In particular, nationalpolls oftenstriveto reporttheopinionsof mem- bersoftheold New Deal coalition,especiallywhena particulargroup seemsto be pivotalin theelectionoutcome.For example,Catholic voterswerepolledafterFordreferred to East Europeansas freefrom Soviet domination, and Jews were polled when New York State appearedcrucialto Carter'ssuccess. Twenty-three percentof the Timesarticlesand 10 percentof thetelevisionnews reportsfocused on thistypeof pollinginformation. Uncertainvoters.Voterswhoidentify withneithertheDemocratic This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 521 northeRepublicanparty-andwhoare therefore unpredictable-may be seen as determiningthe outcome. election Thus on October15,the New YorkTimesfront-page story(Apple, 1976:A1) had thefollowing headlines:"Carter,Focusingon Ford Record,Gains AmongInde- pendentsin Poll." Similarly,"undecided" voters,who are highly volatileandcan decideforeithercandidate,are importantsubjectsfor reporters(see DeVries and Tarrance,1972). The New YorkTimes began its predebatecampaignstorywith the followingheadline: "Large Groupof UndecidedVotersFound Lookingto Debates for Aid: StudyAlso Shows Much of Supportfor Carterand Ford Is Subjectto Quick Change" (Reinhold,1976a). Like undecidedvotersor independents, peoplewho supportthird- partycandidatesmay affectthe resultsand therefore receivegreat attentionfromhorse-race-conscious journalists.Eugene McCarthy was portrayed as a candidatewithsupportamongyoungvoterswho recalledhis anti-Vietnam War positioneightyears earlier.These votersweredisproportionally Democratsand thuscouldconceivably swingto supportCarter.On the otherhand,LesterMaddox could siphonsupportaway fromfellowGeorgianJimmy Carter.Thus,the mainemphasisof a September9 NBC News reportof a Harrispoll (showingCarterin the lead) was the effectof the McCarthyvote, whichmade the outcomeof the electiondifficult to forecast.Simi- larly,Time(1976b)reported on October11that"The Race Turnsinto a Dead Heat" withthefollowing analysis:"The standoffturnedup by the surveyresultedfromaskingthosepolledto choose betweenthe two maincandidates.WhentheYankelovichanalystsfiguredin the effectof the minorcandidates,LesterMaddox and Eugene McCar- thy,FordpulledaheadofCarterby42% to 40%." Fourteenpercentof theNew YorkTimesarticlesand 8 percentof thetelevisionevening news broadcastsaboutpublicopinionpolls focusedon thisformof uncertainty,thusenhancingvoterinterestin the electoralprocess. SHIFTS IN POPULARITY Another techniqueforenhancing journalistic interestthrough polls is comparingcurrentand earlierpoll data. Journalists can choose comparisons in orderto describethecandidatesas close or farapart, butusuallytheirreportsimplyconstantly changing opinion,so thata candidatecan seemto wintheracewitha suddenburstofpopularity. mustdecidewhatto comparetheirdata to: earlierpolls Journalists by the same agency,otherpolls takenat the same time,polls in previousyearsat the same pointin the campaign,and the like. An exampleof a reporter'suse of thisdiscretionto createexcitement This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
522 C. ANTHONY BROH turnedup in an NBC interview withGeorgeGallupon October1, 1976,whichconcludedthatcurrent trendsin Carter'slead overFord would producean election too close to call (VanderbiltTelevision News Archive, 1976).This statement emphasized theshiftin opinion whileignoring theactual11percentdifference betweenthecandidates in Gallup'spoll. A problemwithcomparing pollsat one timeto pollsat anothertime of samplingerror.Since mostcommercial is the interpretation polls havea samplesize of 1,000to 1,500voters,thesampling errorcan be expectedto be about 3 or 4 percent.The actualfigurewilldepend uponthe samplingtechniquesused by theagencyand chancevaria- tion(Weisbergand Bowen, 1977).In recentyearsmostnewspaper accountsof polls reportsamplingerror(Carmody,1976)and sample size; however,thepressrarelyconsidersthisfactorininterpreting the results.5For example,severalarticlesreporteda shiftin popularity measuredby subtractingpopularityat t2frompopularityat t,. Thus a changefrom58 percentto 54 percentwouldhavebeenreported as a 4 percentdecrease. However,the 4 percentmay not signifyattitude changesincea popularity ratingof56 percent,forexample,at botht1 andt2is withinthesampling errorofbothfigures.Thuson October1 theTimesreportshowingan 18percentdifference betweenCarterand Ford at t1,and an 8 percentdifference at t2 may,or maynot,have reflecteda netattitudechangeof 10percent.Nevertheless, theTimes reportedthe figuresas attitudechange. Similarlythe "Fluttering, StutteringPolls" describedby Timemagazine(1976b)in a postelec- tionarticlecouldhave been due to a neglectin considering sampling error. A problemwithcomparing polls of one agencywiththe polls of anotheragencyis the variationin undecidedvoters,whichchanges for several reasons. Some votersdecide how to vote late in the campaign;samplingerrorproducesfluctuations in thepercentageof undecidedvoters;and thepollingagenciesgiveinterviewers different instructionsaboutpressingrespondents foran answer.Speakingbe- foretheAssociatedPressManagingEditorsConference, BurnsRoper noted,"There is also the problemof decidingwhetherto forcethe undecidedvotersto [make]a choice,and,ifso, how and howforce- fullyand whetherto allocatethe remaining undecidedvotersto the candidates"(quotedin Carmody,1976).Clearly,thenumberofunde- S The American AssociationforPublicOpinionResearchhas publishedminimum standardsof disclosurefornews releasesaboutpublicopinionpolls. It urgesnews information mediato includethefollowing whenpreparingfinalcopyforpublicationor broadcast:sponsor,questionwording,population,samplesize, subcategoriesof re- sponse,typeof interview,and timeof survey. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 523 cided voterschangedfrompoll to poll in the 1976campaign.6 These changesmayhave comeaboutbecauseofrandomvariationor biased resultsdue to interviewtechniques.Thatis, theundecidedvotersmay disproportionatelyfavorone candidateoveranother.7 Mostimportant to our discussion,thebias in one samplemaydiffer fromthebias in another.Journalists willreporttheresulting in candidate differences supportas attitudechange,and thusmakea campaignappearmuch morevolatilethanit reallyis. Furthermore, in 1976the selectionof a date forcomparisonpro- duced different imagesof the race. By comparingthe candidates' popularityduringthecampaignto popularity beforeLabor Day,jour- nalistscouldpresenta catch-upimageoftheFordcampaign.By com- paringthe candidates'popularity duringthe campaignto popularity afterLaborDay,journalists couldpresenta nose-to-noseimageofthe campaign.A thirdpossibilitywas to compareCarter'slead in the polls to othercandidates'popularityin previousyears. Franklin Roosevelt'sand RichardNixon'slandslidevictoriesin 1936and 1972, respectively,were typicalpointsof comparison,and were used to emphasizeCarter'sslippagein popularity. Table 1 reportsthe per- centageofarticlesintheNew YorkTimesandtelevision eveningnews storiesaboutpolls whichreporteda shiftin publicopinionin com- parisonto some priorpoint. REPORTING CAMPAIGN EVENTS Another reportingtechniqueforgeneratingexcitement is to empha- size spectaclesor specialeventsduringthecampaign.The New York Timesdid thisin 17 percentof its reports,televisionin 14 percent. The televiseddebates,forexample,showedthe candidatesin face- to-facecompetition, permittingthe armchairjockey to evaluatethe race as itheadedtowardthefinishline.Furthermore, sincecampaign staffsprovidepredebateplansand strategy, and newscommentators emphasizeeach candidate'sviability forofficebased on his perform- ance,themostfeeblecandidatecan appearmorecapable-and thusa close contenderin the election. By reporting figureson publicperceptionsof the debate winner whileignoring figureson overallpublicpreferences, theNew York 6 BetweenLaborDay andElectionDay theTimes reported theresultsof14different nationalpolls. The numberof votersreportedas notfavoringeitherCarteror Ford variedfrom18percentto 7 percent;theseextremeswerereportedon thesameday(see Broh,1979). 7People who respond"I don'tknow" to a surveyquestionare disproportionally nonwhite,low-educated,low-income, and noninvolved (see Francisand Busch,1975; Converse,1976-1977). This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
524 C. ANTHONY BROH Times heightened the citizen'sinvolvement withthe spectacle.In addition,the media emphasizedthe degreeof competition, not the victory.On September 24, theday afterthefirstCarter-Ford debate, NBC reportedtheAssociatedPresspoll callingthedebateslightly in favorofPresidentFord,butemphasizedthat24 percentbelievedthe debateto be a draw.Two dayslaterCBS placedsimilaremphasison the39 percentin itspoll who had no opinionaboutthewinnerofthe debatesor thoughtthe debate was a draw. To the extentthatthe debateschangedvoters'mindsaboutthecandidates,theresultswere "closingthe gap on Mr. Carter"(VanderbiltTelevisionNews Ar- chive,1976:1997). By heightening voterinterest,thisselectiveemphasisin reporting pollsis a valuableaspectof themedia'srole in thedemocraticpro- cess. Butthemediasometimes go beyondwhata democratic theorist mightwish by encouragingvotersto focus on events that affect campaignstrategy but thatmay have littleimpacton a president's behavior.These media-created spectaclesare thenused to analyze trendsor shiftsamongvoters.For example,in an interviewwith Playboymagazine,Jimmy Carterdiscussedhis religionand "lust for women." ObviouslyCarter'ssexual desiresor the factthathe was interviewed in a "sex magazine"wouldnotaffectpolicypositionsor issuesafterthecampaign.However,thesequestionsmayhave influ- encedmanyvotersafterthemediasingledout thePlayboyinterview as an important eventanda cause ofincreasing electoralcompetition. Changesin thepolls,accordingto reporters, resultedfromthispoliti- cal miscalculation.Similarly,GeraldForderredin theseconddebate whenhe said: "I don't believe,Mr. Frankel,thatthe Yugoslavians considerthemselves dominated by the SovietUnion.I don'tbelieve thatthe Poles considerthemselvesdominatedby the SovietUnion. Each of these countriesis independent or autonomous"(Congres- sional Quarterly Almanac, 1976:930).Afterthegaffe,publicopinion pollsreported thatCatholicsof East Europeanancestrywerereturn- ingto theDemocraticranks.The highconcentration of Catholicsin Detroit,Chicago, Cleveland,and Milwaukeecould have affected Ford's chances in Michigan,Illinois,Ohio, and Wisconsin-states crucialto GOP strategy. By focusingon event-linked changesin the polls,themediaencouragedcitizensto weighstatements in termsof theireffecton the campaign,not on UnitedStatesforeignpolicy. Eventsoccurring duringthecampaignaffecta candidate'ssupport and thushis positionin the race. Each mistake,each appearance, each stumbleis important because it affectsthecandidate'sstrategy and mighteven cause his campaignto falterfora moment.By em- phasizingthese events reportersfocus on the volatilityof public This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 525 opinion,theunpredictability oftherace,and thesignificance ofcam- paignstatementsratherthanactualbehavior. INACCURACIES IN REPORTING In a fewinstances,minorinaccuraciesoccurredin thereporting of polls,and alwaysin a mannerthatindicateda close race. For exam- ple,on September 2, David Brinkleyreported thelatestGalluppollas givingCarter52 percentand Ford 37 percent.The Gallup Organ- ization'spostelectionreportshowedthe resultsof the same poll as 54-36 (Gallup,1976:13),a 3 percentwiderdifference thanthe NBC spread.Timemagazinereported a YankelovichpolloflateSeptember as a tie, 43 percentto 43 percent(Time,1976b:16). A monthlater, Time's(1976a:17)retrospective analysisshowedthatFordactuallyled inthepoll,43 percentto 41 percent.Although thedifference was only in allocationof the McCarthyvote, the reportconfoundedthe in- terpretation of a 4 percentlead takenby Carterin the following month'spoll,and negatedthecontention oftheSeptemberstorythat the campaignwas a "dead heat." Similarly,the New YorkTimes exaggerated theeffectofthefirstdebatein itsreportof a Galluppoll showingan 8 percentdifference betweenthe trial-heat popularityof Carterand Ford.The poll,takenSeptember 24 to 27 andthefirstafter thedebates,was highlyinfluential in producing theillusionthatFord was "catchingup." The October1 New YorkTimes(1976a:8)story hadthefollowing lead: "CarterMarginin GallupPoll Is Cutto 50-42. The latestGallupPoll on thePresidential contestshowsthatJimmy Carter'slead overPresidentFordhas beenreducedfrom18 percent- age pointsto 8." Gallup's(1976:13)postelection analysisreportsthe poll resultsas 51 to 40 percent. Withno attempt to deceiveor misleadvoters,thecumulative effect oftheseindividually trivialinaccuraciesis nonetheless to heightenthe senseof competition and to sharpentheimageof a horserace. They are perhapsmostimportant as a demonstration of the unconscious powerofthedriveto maintain voter-andreaderor viewer-interest. IGNORING RELEVANT DATA Stillanothertechniquefordealingwithpoll data is to ignoreor downplaythem,especiallyifthepoll showsthecandidatesapart.For example,theNew YorkTimesand CBS commissioned severalpolls duringthe 1976presidential campaign,one of whichwas conducted betweenOctober8 and 12 and releasedto thepublicon October15. The GallupOrganizationalso conducteda poll duringthe same pe- This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
526 C. ANTHONY BROH riod, October 8 throughOctober 11. The Times (Apple, 1976) empha- sized the six-pointdifferencereportedby Gallup and played down its own poll which made the race seem slightlyless competitive.The fourthparagraphof the page-one storyread as follows: 6 Point Edge for Carter A new nationalsurveyby the Galluporganization, the resultsof which weredisclosedyesterday, gave Mr. Cartertheedge by 6 percentage points, 48 to 42. A simultaneous nationalpoll by PatrickCaddellforMr. Carterput his marginat 8 points.The Times/CBSfindings, whichwerebased on tele- phoneinterviews with1,761registered votersbetweenOctober8 and 12,fell intothe same generalrange. The lead sentence suggeststhatthe 6 percentedge is "correct," while other percentages are either biased-since Caddell worked for Carter-or close enoughto be "the same." However, theTimes's own poll showed 44 percent for Carter and 37 percent for Ford-a 7- percentdifference.8 While 7 percentis clearlywithinthe range of the Gallup poll difference,theTimes reportedand emphasized the smaller figureof an outside polling agency. Moreover, the referenceto the Times poll which emphasized its methodology (" 1,761 registered voters between October 8 and 12") had the effectof bolsteringthe reliabilityof the Gallup poll, whose sample size it did not report. NBC News explicitlyignoredthe poll data in its September16 news report(VanderbiltTelevision News Archive, 1976:1941). JohnChan- cellor announcedthatvice presidentialcandidate WalterMondale was campaigningaround the country,andthen stated, "Despite public opinionpolls, Carterand Mondale may be in a tightrace." The story that followed showed Mondale on the campaign trailand Democrats and labor leaders saying the race was too close for comfortin their states of Ohio, California,Michigan, Indiana, New Jersey,and Il- linois. The storyimpliesthatlocal leaders "know" how close the race really is while the national polls are misleading.This kind of report did not appear later in the campaign when the nationalpolls showed the candidates to be very close. Conclusionsand Discussion The horse-race metaphor of campaigns is not new. The Boston Journal in 1888 proclaimedthata "dark horse" is unlikelyto emerge fromthe campaign. In 1924, the New York Times ran the following 8 Information received by the authorin a telephoneconversationwithDwight Mor- ris, March 20, 1980. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 527 headlineabout the Democraticconvention:"With 21 Candidates, It Is Now The Field AgainstM'Adoo" (Keylin and Nelson, 1976). What is noteworthyis that polling technologyof computer-drawnsamples, random digit dialing, and carefullyconstructedquestions has con- formedso easily to the traditionalreportingtechniquesofjournalists. Viewing a presidentialcampaign as a horse race, then, has a long tradition.A democratic theoristmightdecry this rathercrude con- nectionbetween a sporting,indeed a betting,event, and the ideals of democracy. Where in all of this is Rousseau's informedand commit- ted participantin the events that shape his life,Jefferson'sfreedom- loving farmer,de Tocqueville's equality-lovingentrepreneur,V. 0. Key's rationalvoter? But it is one thingto uphold democraticideals; it is a verydifferent thingto condemnthe media forusing a sporting image to frametheiranalyses of elections. The horse-race metaphorin fact has several valuable functions. First, and most important,it enhances the public's interest in a process that could seem remote,mysterious,and boring. If citizens are more attractedto sports than to politics, why not use sports to teach them about politics? Because it makes the electoral process appear as excitingand competitiveas possible, the horse-raceimage maintainsa vital link between the mass of people and a very few elected officials.Second, the horse-race image induces reportersto focus on polls that hold great interestfor special groups of voters. Reportingthe views of Californians,or Catholics, or proponentsof gun controlmay not give us much sense of how our candidate is doing overall, but it is of vital importance to westerners, the religious community,and gun owners. Finally, the horse-raceimage mitigates the danger inherentin the very existence of the mass media-that reporterscould have much more influencethan they or others want by settlingon a winnerearly in the process and turningthe campaign into a self-fulfilling prophecy. The horse-race image encourages re- portersto emphasize competitionratherthan to forecast results. However, we mustconclude witha note of caution. My analysis of the 1976 election shows two problems in the use of the horse-race metaphor.First,reporterswho seek a themeto make a storyexciting may inadvertently distortpolls to the pointof nonsense. Burns Roper (1976) illustratedthe discrepanciesin the use of his poll in a satirical letterto the editor of the New York Times: To theEditor: ... It had notoccurredto meto combinefromour"instant"debatepollthe 31 percentwhothought GovernorCarterwonwiththe30 percentwhofeltit was a drawand conclude". . . a totalof 61 percentof thosepolledthought thatthe debatewas a drawor a Cartervictory."Nor was I perspicacious This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
528 C. ANTHONY BROH enoughto recognizethat"in politicalterms,"thatwas an excellentresultfor thenomineeofthestronger party.[However,I becamemoreconfusedwhenI read]thatsincethePresident'sstrength priorto thedebatewas onlyabout33 percent(moreor less, dependingon whichpoll you read),thedebatewas a victoryforthe Presidentsince his win-plus-draw figurewas 61 percentac- cordingto theAssociatedPresspoll-roughlytwicethepercentage preferring him prior to the debate. . . . I had looked simplisticallyat our 39 percent Ford,31 percentCarter,30 percentdrawfiguresand concludedthatPresi- dentFordhas a smalledge.It is nowobviousto me thatitwas eithera clear victoryforGovernorCarteror an overwhelming winforPresidentFord. To Roper, scientificallydrawn samples are more credible thanjour- nalists' impressions-except when analystsuse the polls to see what- ever they like. Polls are as credible as their users; the search for excitementcarries withinit the danger of distortion. Finally, the horse-race image can encourage voters to focus on exciting, but ultimatelyirrelevantaspects of a campaign. Just as bettors on a race may be misled by the beauty of the horses- champion thoroughbredsare often the least showy-voters may be misled by the triviaof a campaign. Pseudo-eventsthatare created by the media, and whose effectson poll resultsare thenanalyzed by the same media forclues to the dynamicsof a campaign,do the voters a disservice. Conversely,issues which are complex, not hotlydebated, but more significant,may be passed over in the search forexcitement. In sum, the horse-race image frames the analysis of polls in an election with generallybeneficialand sometimesdeleterious effects. By enhancingthe excitementof the campaign, the media draw the electoratecloser to theirelected representatives,withonly occasional costs to the integrity of the poll data or the ideal democraticprocess. References Apple,R. W. 1976 "Carter,focusingon Ford record,gains amongindependents in poll." New YorkTimes, Oct. 15, pp. Alff. Broh,C. A. 1979 PublicOpinion,Pollingand thePress:The Race forthePresidency. Durham,N.C.: Duke University Instituteof Policy Sciences and PublicAffairsWorkingPaper. 1981 "Whetherbellwethers or weather-jars predictelections."Western PoliticalQuarterly (forthcoming). Cantril,A. H. 1976 "The press and the pollster."Annalsof the Academyof Political and Social Science 427:45-52. Carmody,D. 1976 "Poll takersvoice some reservations in analysisof coverageby newspapersof campaignpolls." New YorkTimes,Nov. 13, p. 21. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
HORSE-RACE JOURNALISM 529 CongressionalQuarterlyAlmanac 1976 Washington, Quarterly. D.C.: Congressional Converse,JeanM. 1976- "Predictingno opinionin the polls." Public OpinionQuarterly 1977 40:515-30. DeVries,W., and L. Tarrance GrandRapids:WilliamB. Eerdmans. 1972 The Ticket-Splitter. Francis,Joe D., and LawrenceBusch 1975 "Whatwe nowknowabout'I don'tknows'." PublicOpinionQuar- terly39:207-18. Gallup,G. H. 1976 "Presidentialtrialheats,1976." The GallupOpinionIndex. Report No. 137:13. Keylin,Arleen,and Eve Nelson 1976 If Elected . . . New York: Arno Press. New York Times 1976a"Cartermarginin Galluppoll is cut to 50-42." Oct. 1, p. 8. 1976b"Poll showsslidein Cartermargin."Aug. 25, p. 1. Reinhold,R. 1976a "Large groupofundecidedvotersfoundlookingto debateforaid." New YorkTimes,Sept. 22, p. 24. 1976b"SurveyshowsCarterholdslead becausehe is seen as moderate." New York Times,Oct. 19,p. 32. Rokeach,M. 1968 "The role of values in publicopinionresearch."Public Opinion Quarterly32:547-59. Roper,Bums 1976 "Letters." New YorkTimes,Oct. 7, p. 46. Time 1976a"Cartertakesa narrowlead." Nov. 1, pp. 17-18. 1976b"The race turnsintoa dead heat." Oct. 1, pp. 16-17. VanderbiltTelevisionNews Archive 1976 Televisionnews index and abstracts.Nashville:JointUniversity Libraries. Weisberg,HerbertF., and BruceD. Bowen 1977 An Introductionto SurveyResearchand Data Analysis.San Fran- cisco: W. H. Freeman. This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
American Association for Public Opinion Research Horse-Race Journalism: Reporting the Polls in the 1976 Presidential Election Author(s): C. Anthony Broh Source: The Public Opinion Quarterly, Vol. 44, No. 4, Polls and the News Media: A Symposium (Winter, 1980), pp. 514-529 Published by: Oxford University Press on behalf of the American Association for Public Opinion Research Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/2748469 . Accessed: 27/01/2015 12:06 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact support@jstor.org. . American Association for Public Opinion Research and Oxford University Press are collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to The Public Opinion Quarterly. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 128.220.159.74 on Tue, 27 Jan 2015 12:06:58 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions
You can also read