ETHNOLINGUISTIC VITALITY AND MINORITY EDUCATION
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
JoLIE 2:1 (2009) ETHNOLINGUISTIC VITALITY AND MINORITY EDUCATION Martin Ehala University of Tartu, Finland Abstract To respect and protect linguistic and cultural diversity is one of the core principles of UNESCO and EU. Yet globalisation has considerably challenged the sustainability of small ethnic and linguistic communities. Increased mobility, concentration of wealth, and new information technologies are inevitably working towards the decrease of cultural and linguistic diversity. However, the maintenance of diversity depends also heavily on various cultural and social psychological factors that could be influenced by education and mass communication. The paper outlines the main social psychological factors influencing the ethnolinguistic vitality of a culture or ethnic group and reveals their structural relationships, for example the prestige of the culture, cultural distance and utilitarianism and discordance. Based on this model, suggestions are made how to enhance the ethnolinguistic vitality of lesser used languages and cultures in the broad framework of bilingual and minority education.1. Key words: Ethnolinguistic vitality; Multiple identity; Self-categorisation. 1. Introduction One of the core principles of UNESCO, the Council of Europe and EU is to respect and protect linguistic and cultural diversity. The “UNESCO Universal Declaration on Cultural Diversity“, adopted by the UNESCO’s General Conference in November 2001, declares that “as a source of exchange, innovation and creativity, cultural diversity is as necessary for humankind as biodiversity is for nature. In this sense, it is the common heritage of humanity and should be recognized and affirmed for the benefit of present and future generations“. The Article 22 of the Charter of Fundamental Rights of the European Union, adopted in Nice on 7 December 2000, states that the „Union shall respect cultural, religious and linguistic diversity". The Council of European Union’s resolution of 14 February 2002 on the promotion of linguistic diversity and language learning in the framework of the implementation of the objectives of the European Year of Languages 2001 emphasises that „all European languages are equal in value and dignity from the cultural point of view and form an integral part of European culture and civilisation“. 1 This paper is a part of the project “Ethnolinguistic vitality and identity construction: Estonia in Baltic background” supported by Estonian Science Foundation grant no 7350.
38 Martin EHALA Yet at our time, the time of the global village, to protect cultural and linguistic diversity is not an easy task. Admittedly one language becomes extinct in each week. Although there are at present around 6000 to 7000 languages, from 50% to 90% of them could become extinct within this century (see Krauss 1992, Crystal 2000). While there is some international recognition that a few domains such as science and higher education might be threatened in national languages (Communiqué 2003: 6), generally the endangered languages are those that have neither official status nor legal protection. While it is important to work for legal protection of minority languages, it may not be enough: in our era of global information exchange, the fate of a language may depend less of its legal status than the attitudes of its speakers. There are a number of known cases where the linguists have been more concerned by protecting an endangered language than the speakers themselves. Generally, such attempts have doomed to fail: unless the attitudes of the speakers do not favour language maintenance, it is little what the specialists can do to protect the language. The question that needs to be answered is whether there is anything that could be done to change the negative attitudes of an endangered language. To answer this question one needs to understand the nature of ethnolinguistic vitality and how it is formed. This knowledge could then be purposefully used in minority language education in order to promote a change in attitudes. 2. The nature of ethnolinguistic vitality 2.1. The definition of ethnolinguistic vitality Ethnolinguistic vitality is usually defined as what “makes a group likely to behave as a distinctive and active collective entity in intergroup situations.” (Giles, Bourhis and Taylor 1977: 308). Deep down, it is a group identity issue. According to Tajfel (1978: 63), social identity is “that part of an individual's self-concept which derives from his knowledge of his membership of a social group (or groups) together with the value and emotional significance attached to that membership.” Ellemers, Kortekaas and Ouwerkerk (1999: 386) argue that these three components of social identity – cognitive, evaluative and emotional – are conceptually distinct aspects of identity; and that only the “group commitment appears to be the key aspect of social identity which drives the tendency for people to behave in terms of their group membership.” Portes and Sensenbrenner (1993) call this commitment bounded solidarity, which is the feeling of unity that often arises from real or perceived threats to the group. Thus, ethnolinguistic vitality could be defined as the group’s will to act collectively, deriving from its members’ emotional attachment to this particular group membership. The stronger the affective commitment is, the more vitality the group possesses. Of course, the affective commitment is not the sole cause that motivates group members to behave collectively in intergroup situations. It is often that people are morally forced to participate in the actions of the group even if they do not approve of the action. Portes and Sensenbrenner (1993) refer to this phenomenon as
Ethnolinguistic vitality and minority education 39 enforceable trust, which is defined as the group’s capacity to sanction those who do not obey the norms. To be more precise, bounded solidarity and enforceable trust could be seen as two facets of what Bourdieu (1991: 170) calls symbolic power – a power of “making people see and believe, of conforming or transforming the vision of the world and, thereby, action on the world … by virtue of the specific effect of mobilization … capable of producing real effects without apparent expenditure of energy.” All of the factors mentioned above – emotional attachment, bonded solidarity, enforceable trust and symbolic power are all built communicatively in public and private discourses. Educational discourse is one of the main tools that is used by groups to construct social cohesion and rise ethnolinguistic vitality. In order to influence these discourses it is necessary to know how precisely ethnolinguistic vitality is generated what are its main components. These questions have found a an answer in the V-model (Ehala 2005, 2008) that specifies the structural relations between four key variables of ethnolinguistic vitality: perceived strength differential between a group and its most prominent outgroup (PSD); perceived inter-group distance (R); the level of utilitarianism (U) in the value system of the group studied; and the level of inter-group discordance (D). 2.2 Perceived strength differential Most researchers agree that the driving force behind language shift is power differences between the dominant and minority groups and that identity maintenance or shift depends on the opportunities and rewards, real or symbolic (including more positive social identity) that the two groups can provide to their members. I have called all these factors together as the cultural mass (M) of the group (see Ehala 2005). However, for group vitality, the crucial factor is not the cultural mass itself, but the differential of cultural masses between the two groups – the ingroup and the prominent outgroup. If the cultural mass differential (PSD) is small, the benefits from shifting one’s group membership would not outweigh its emotional and social costs. The larger the PSD is in favor of the outgroup, the more beneficial it would be to shift the identity. Needless to say, what counts is the subjective perception of, or to be more precise, the socially shared vision concerning this difference. Thus, provided that the influence of all other factors is absent, the vitality (V) of the group would be equal to the differential of the cultural masses (M1 and M2) of the minority (G1) and majority (G2) groups: (1) V = S1 – S2 If V
40 Martin EHALA 2.3. Inter-group distance Although the PSD is the driving force behind identity and language shift, it is hardly unaffected by other factors that either hinder or enhance this tendency. One such factor is inter-group distance (R). This is a complex factor that refers to the various components making up group boundaries defined as “patterns of social interaction that give rise to, and subsequently reinforce, in-group members’ self-identification and outsiders’ confirmation of group distinctions.” (Sanders 2002: 327). A loss of vitality follows the loosening of these patterns and their replacement by patterns that work for some other identity. Thus, all factors being otherwise equal, the vitality of the group is higher the larger the perceived distance between the groups is, i.e. the clearer the groups’ boundaries are and the more distinct the groups appear. As intergroup distance is a complex notion, it can be divided into two subfactors: extent of intergroup contact and cultural distinctiveness. The former expresses the minority’s ability to maintain their networks while the environment offers opportunities for the development of a different network that unavoidably would loosen the strength of the heritage network (Landry, Allard and Henry 1996). Sanders (2002) refers to numerous cases where ethnic entrepreneurship was able to provide resources for the community, thus reducing the need for contacts with outside communities. The segregative networks created and maintained by this process have been shown to enhance the vitality of the group despite large differentials between the cultural masses. Even in cases where intergroup contacts eventually become more widespread and “acculturation moves forward, some aspects of assimilation are resisted by groups who have developed effective social networks and institutions for generating and distributing scarce resources to group members” (Sanders 2002: 333). Network structure in turn is heavily interrelated with language usage: as intergroup contact often involves two languages, the network structure will determine the language usage patterns. The tenser the contacts with the dominant outgroup are the more the dominant language is used. This means that the language usage pattern is often a good indicator of the extent of intergroup contact. On the other hand, language is also a boundary feature for many groups and as such indicates the cultural distinctiveness of the group (Barth 1969). Cultural distinctiveness is determined by the number and vividness of the boundary features of the group. Some of the features are essentialist by nature such as racial features; some are socially constructed such as religion. While latter features could be abandoned by group members, the essential features may not. Language, although a constructed feature has strong essentialist characteristics – the native accent is hard to conceal. Often language is the defining boundary feature for a group (see also the notion of core values of Smolics 1981; Smolics and Secombe 1989), but the boundary could also be a marked by other features such as religion and related cultural practices (Myhill 2003). The vitality of the group further depends on its cultural distinctiveness – the number of features defining the group and the extent of essentialism of these features. Sanders (2002: 342) refers to a number of studies indicating the inhibitory
Ethnolinguistic vitality and minority education 41 effect that individuals’ distinctive racial features have on their choice of possible ethnic identities. Ultimately, the intergroup distance is determined by symbolic and discursive factors of vitality that set the norms concerning the acceptability, extent and nature of intergroup contacts, as well as the wish to maintain cultural distinctiveness. Hornsey and Hogg (2000: 147) report a number of cases where a perceived threat to identity brought in by gradual convergence in intergroup contact has eventually led to symbolic actions to reduce intergroup contact and to stress cultural differences. So, the intergroup distance could be an effective factor affecting a group’s vitality. Mathematically, its relation to the other factors could be expressed as in (2): (2) V = (S1 – S2)/R The minimal value for R is 1. This would correspond to minimal intergroup distance both in terms of social network terms and cultural distinctiveness. In such cases, r has no impact on the vitality V which is determined only by the PSD. When R is larger than 1, it starts to reduce the negative cultural mass differential and by this V starts to approach zero. The larger R gets, the closer V gets to zero, i.e. the point of ethnolinguistic stability. 2.4 Utilitarianism Utilitarianism is a broad discursive mindset that justifies pragmatic and economically beneficial courses of action. According to Scollon and Scollon (1995) the basic principles of utilitarian discourse are as follows: 1) humans are defined as rational economic entities, 2) ‘good’ is defined as what will give the greatest happiness for the greatest number, and 3) values are established by statistical (i.e. quantitative) means. Each culture, though, functions as an interplay of innovation and tradition and the utilitarian principles are balanced by what could be called the identity discourse: 1) the essence of humanity is emotional; 2) the notion of “good” is set by the moral authority; 3) values are defined by tradition. The success of identity discourse relies on the emotional attachment of a person to their group members and heritage, as well as to their immediate surroundings – the cultural landscape. In a balanced culture, the utilitarian discourse and the identity discourse are in a modest conflict of innovation and tradition, which is a characteristic of many well- functioning societies. As the utilitarian principles are discursive, different groups may differ in respect to the salience of the utilitarian principles in their culture. The less salient these principles are the more conservative the culture is. For example some religious groups (like the Amish or Russian old-believers) are so conservative that they almost do not assimilate at all despite the large negative PSD. In this case the utilitarianism is virtually zero. If the society is totally utilitarian, the assimilative effects of the cultural mass differential are enforced; and if the utilitarian and identity discourses are well balanced, U does not affect the impact of PSD on vitality. From this, it could be reasonable to let the values of U to change in the diapason 0≤U≥2 and to include it in the formula in the following way:
42 Martin EHALA (3) V=U(S1-S2)/R This means that if the value of U is 1, its impact to the overall vitality can be disregarded. If its value is greater than 1, the effects of PSD start to be increased. In the case the U value reaches its maximum value 2, the effect of PSD gets doubled. In the case of a typical minority group, which has a negative PSD, high U value causes it to increase. The reverse is also true: if the value of U falls below 1, it starts to reduce the negative value of PSD. When U reaches 0, the whole equation becomes equal to 0, indicating that this particular group is stable due to its extremely traditional and conservative value system. 2.5 Inter-group discordance Intergroup discordance, or D-factor, expresses perceived illegitimacy of intergroup power relations as well as the dehumanisation of the outgroup. Although legitimacy and stigmatisation are clearly distinct concepts, they are interrelated. A pilot study conducted in Estonia to design an instrument for measuring D-factor revealed that, both amongst the ethnic Estonians as the majority and ethnic Russians as a minority, the perceived legitimacy of the interethnic situation in Estonia and the level of stigmatisation of the outgroup were statistically significantly correlated: for Estonians r = 0,202 (P
Ethnolinguistic vitality and minority education 43 large and situation is considered legitimate. This would signify lower vitality. When the D-factor is incorporated, the V formula obtains the following form: (1) V=U((S1-S2)+D)/R This means that we can say that the low vitality of the minority group depends on the perceived high stability of its low status in the situation where the intergroup distance r is small (that reduces the costs of identity shift) and the attitudes of the members of the group are utilitarian (which favours social mobility and identity shift). The vitality of the minority group is thus higher, the more unstable the situation is perceived; the larger the intergroup distance are and the more prominent the traditional and conservative values. An interesting feature is that V is culture specific. There is no objective value for V; it can only be calculated from the point of view of G1 to have some predictive force for this group’s behaviour. The reason is that assimilative processes are made on an individual level by the people who belong to the same group G1. This means that all the values of the variables in this formula are also culture specific, not objective. As the parameters are subjective, their values are to some extent variable: they depend on the particulars of cultural discourse in G1. By gradually changing the discourse, it is possible to increase the perceived strength of the group (i.e. S1) or to reduce the value of U, R or D. For this, it is important to know the discursive processes that govern this process. 3. Promoting ethnolinguistic vitality According to the hypothesis, presented above, the main tool for protecting linguistic and cultural diversity is ultimately discursive: constructing a positive group identity, (increasing S1), enhancing its cultural distinctiveness (increasing R), rising in-group loyalty and solidarity (reducing U) or to rise the level of intergroup discordance (D). Of course, some would at this point certainly see here a ghost of xenophobia, intolerance and violence that attempts have generated. For example Mikael Hjerm (1998) argues, relying on the Swedish and Norwegian data from the 1995 International Social Survey Programme (ISSP) “Aspects of national identity”, that nationalist sentiments are in a strong correlation with xenophobic attitudes. Based on these findings, he calls into question any possibilities of constructing ‘good’ nationalism. He states that “first, even if nationalism is based on national independence the reason for this independence often boils down to that the people of one nation imagine that there is a difference between themselves and some other people and that in the long run they are better off with their own nation-state. Therefore, they in some sense are superior to other peoples and nations” (Hjerm 1998: 52). One has to agree that the manipulation of D has in the history of humankind been used by far too often and with too dangerous consequences and should be avoided as discordance has tendency towards exponential growth. This means that a careless attempt to slightly increase a group’s vitality goes quickly out of hands and
44 Martin EHALA leads to a full blown interethnic conflict. However the other variables in the model are much stable, although a manipulation of these could increase xenophobia and out-group discrimination. The question is whether there is a possibility to avoid it. 3.1. Optimal intergroup distinctiveness Social psychological research on minimal groups (i.e. arbitrary groups, created for the experiment only, groups that have no history, no shared beliefs etc) has shown that even ad hoc group categorisation leads to in-group favouritism and out-group discrimination (Tajfel 1970). The same results were obtained even when the assignment to the groups was made explicitly random (Billig and Tajfel 1973). This indicates that out-group discrimination is a phenomenon that is inherent in the intergroup situation itself (Tajfel and Turner 1979). If the in-group bias is indeed so universal, the behaviour may well have biological roots. Be as it may, so long as humans have social groups of any type, there will always be in-group bias. This may not necessarily be a bad thing. As Tajfel and Turner (1979) argue, individuals strive to sustain or enhance their self-respect. Social groups however may have either high or low prestige, and thus, by comparing one’s own group with reference to other relevant groups, one tries to maintain a positive social identity. Accordingly, the in-group bias is a natural result of a person’s positive self-esteem. There is little hope to get rid of it, there can only be possibilities to find an optimal balance between the respect towards one’s own social identity and the respect towards others. I call this optimally intergroup distinctiveness (OID). The hypothesis is that groups with OID will maintain healthy and stable intergroup relations in contact settings. Although the in-group bias is a natural phenomenon, it is possibly also dangerous as it is hard to determine at which point the OID is exceeded and the group is constructing a conflictual identity discourse, particularly as the in-group favouritism is often employed by politicians to mobilise the masses for social change. This is especially evident when the identity discourse is constructed on a single feature, be it ethnicity, religion, social class or gender. Identities constructed on one feature are inherently more conflictual, as they do not allow overlapping of group membership. In reality individuals belong simultaneously into different social groups and have potentially multiple social identities. This is particularly true for minorities. 3.2. Developing multiple identities One of the most effective ways to promote the perceived strength of the ingroup is to use the notion of multiple identities. According to Barvosa-Carter (1999: 113) multiple identity is „a concept in which the self is made up of a number of different but integrated identities. Each identity is a frame of reference that includes a scheme of values and a set of meanings and practices. These identity frames of reference (or identity frames) are related to a nearly endless array of possible social identities“. An important feature is that people use their partial identities situationally and relationally, i.e they foreground the identity that best serves their immediate needs in that particular situation and the one which is shared with others in this
Ethnolinguistic vitality and minority education 45 context. This phenomenon is called identity adaptiveness and the empirical social psychological research has shown that people tacitly make good use of the positive stereotypes that are associated with some of their multiple identities, depending on situation (Pittinski, Shih and Ambady 1999). Of course, if negative stereotypes of one’s social identity is made salient in a situation where these stereotypes are relevant, this debilitates performance. For example Steele and Aronson (1995) found that African American students underperformed in a verbal test when the stereotypes about black people were made salient. Thus, multiple identity is beneficial in two respects, first, it reduces the possibility of confrontational social distinctiveness and it enables individuals to optimally perform in different social contexts and situations. Constructing multiple identities is a communicative process by which shared social beliefs are generated over various social categories. For some categories the identity construction is more intense, for some other categories less so. Consequently, the corresponding identities have different salience within the totality of one’s multiple identity. To promote multiple identity, the main features of a given group’s identity discourse should be analysed and assessed about the content, whether positive or negative symbols prevail, what is their meaning and possible impact on group behaviour. Based on this analysis the curriculum and its content should be modified for OID. There are a few central themes that help to promote vitality. First, it is necessary to give a meaning to the existence of this group in this particular setting and by this a hope for the brighter future. One should analyse what could be the raison d’etre of this group. For this the available cultural material myths, literature, customs, beliefs etc should be analysed to find motives that could rise group members’ pride in their group membership. If necessary, it should be invented. Most of the successful nations have constructed their identity to a large extent artificially, using available cultural material. Minority language educationalists and activists should engage in a similar type of social creativity to promote perceived group strength. Second, the concept of salvation, the turn of the status should be provided. Hope should be given that there is something by which the group could become great and enhance it status. Either task is not an easy one, as the small low status groups usually have a shortage of suitable cultural material. Parallel with the search for the raison d’etre, ways to signal group membership should be promoted. This, of course depends on the level of existing intergroup distance configuration. If a group is already very distinctive and may even be stigmatised for this distinctiveness, ways should be found how to change the value of this distinctiveness. Instead of being ashamed of these features, a conscious discourse should be created that values these features as positive. A prime example here is the “Black is beautiful” movement in US in the early 60ies. Third line of work should be directed towards reducing utilitarianism in the value system. Utilitarianism is the main cause that makes the minority members to opt for social mobility for personal advancement. This itself is not to be condemned as it is also a source for wealth and self esteem. The crucial factor here is to combine utilitarianism
46 Martin EHALA and traditionalism. As recent studies have shown (Ehala forthcoming), utilitarianism and traditionalism are not correlated, but independent. This means that for supporting utilitarianism, one needs to reject traditionalism. Empirical evidence shows that both can be easily accommodated. One of the goals of minority education is to promote traditionalism, attachment of heritage cultural values while also supporting utilitarianism. In fact, modern welfare society promotes utilitarianism itself. It is important not to criticise utilitarianism as something that brings decay – these arguments won’t work for people who strive for better life. The goal here needs to be explaining that being utilitarian does not force one to abandon heritage. If this goal is achieved, a substantial step in promoting vitality has been made. 4. Conclusion Influencing the identity discourse is not an easy task, as there are multiple societal forces that are in a constant work in maintaining or modifying it. These forces may have partly different views about the identity of the group, its position and future development. Also, the cultural patterns are not easily changed. Thus, it is not at all guaranteed that such attempts through the minority education would be successful. One that must be taken into account is that the process should be very subtle and long-lasting. As said, societal beliefs do not change easily, and every attempt to force a change is most likely to have the opposite effect. This article outlined some basic processes that have effect on intergroup relations. By knowing and using these processes, it is in principle possible to design the identity discourse disseminated through minority. How this is to be done, and how much intervention is needed, could only be decided on each particular case separately, taking into account the cultural and historical circumstances. For this the first step should be to study the particular minority group to find out what its vitality is and particularly what the values are for S1, U, R and D. Using the V- model for obtaining a synopsis of the actual vitality of a group would be the first step. The V-model is fully operationalised for quantitative research (see Ehala and Niglas 2007; Ehala 2008) and could be easily applicable to any minority. On the basis of research, it could be decided where intervention is needed and how to conduct it. In any case, this is a vast task, demanding extensive work on analysing the particular identity discourse and also a fair amount of creativeness to find the best symbolic expression for the features to be introduced or changed. Editors’ note: This article is the author’s contribution to the Proceedings of the Exploratory Workshop Linguistic and Intercultural Education in the Process of Europeanisation of Higher Education CLIE-2009, Popescu, T & Pioariu R (Eds.). Aeternitas: Alba- Iulia, pp. 136-147 (reproduced with permission).
Ethnolinguistic vitality and minority education 47 References Bar-Tal, D. (2000). Shared beliefs in a society. Social psychological analysis. Thousand Oaks, London: Sage. Barth, F. (1969). Ethnic groups and boundaries. In F. Barth (Ed.), Ethnic groups and boundaries: The social organisation of culture difference (pp. 9-37). London: George Allen and Unwin. Barvosa-Carter, E. (1999). Multiple identity and coalition building: How identity differences within us enable radical alliances among us. Contemporary Justice Review, 2, 111-126. Billig. M. & Tajfel, H. (1973). Social categorization and similarity in intergroup behaviour. European Journal of Social Psychology, 3, 27-52. Bourdieu, P. (1991). Language and symbolic power. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Harvard University Press. Crystal, D. (2000). Language death. Cambridge; New York: Cambridge University Press. Ehala, M. (2005). The role of MTE in language maintenance and developing multiple identities. In S. Kiefer & K. Sallamaa (Eds.), European identities in mother tongue education (pp. 36-50). Trauner Verlag: Linz. Ehala, M. (2008). An evaluation matrix for ethno-linguistic vitality. In S. Pertot, T. Priestly, & C. Williams (Eds.), Rights, Promotion and Integration Issues for Minority Languages in Europe (pp. 123-137). Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan Ltd. Ehala, M. & Niglas, K. (2007). Empirical evaluation of a mathematical model of ethnolinguistic vitality: The case of Võro. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 28, 427-444. Ellemers, N., Kortekaas, P., & Ouwerkerk, J.W. (1999). Self-categorisation, commitment to the group and group self-esteem as related but distinct aspects of social identity. European Journal of Social Psychology 29, 371-389 European University Association (2003). Realising the European Higher Education Area. Communiqué of the Conference of Ministers responsible for Higher Education in Berlin on 19 September 2003. Giles, H., Bourhis, R.Y., & Taylor, D.M. (1977). Towards a theory language in ethnic group relations. In H. Giles (Ed.), Language, ethnicity and intergroup relations (pp. 307-348). London: Academic Press. Hjerm, M. (1998). Reconstructing "Positive" Nationalism: Evidence from Norway and Sweden. Sociological Research Online, 3(2). Retrieved February 17, 2009, from http://www.socresonline.org.uk/3/2/7.html. Hornsey, M.J., & Hogg, M.A. (2000). Assimilation and diversity: An integrative model of subgroup relations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 4, 143-156.
48 Martin EHALA Krauss, M. (1992). The world’s languages in crisis. Language, 68, 4-10. Landry, R., Allard, R., & Henry, J. (1996). French in South Louisiana: Towards language loss. Journal of Multilingual and Multicultural Development, 17, 442-468. Myhill, J. (2003). The native speaker, identity, and the authenticity hierarchy. Language Sciences, 25, 77–97. Pittinsky, T.L., Shih, M., & Ambady, N. (1999). Identity adaptiveness: Affect across multiple identities. Journal of Social Issues, 3, 503-518. Portes A., & Sensenbrenner, J. (1993). Embeddedness and immigration: notes on the social determinants of economic action. American Journal of Sociology, 98, 1320–50. Sanders, J.M. (2002). Ethnic boundaries and identity in plural societies. Annual Revue of Sociology 28, 327–57. Scollon, R. & Scollon, S. (1995). Intercultural communication: A discourse approach. Oxford: Blackwell. Smith, A.D. (1999). Myths and memories of the nation. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Smolicz, J.J. (1981). The three types of multiculturalism. In M. Garner (Ed.), Community languages - their role in education (pp. 1-12). Melbourne & Sydney: River Seine Publications. Smolicz, J.J., & Secombe, M.J. (1989). Types of language activation and evaluation in an ethnically plural society. In W. Ammon (Ed.), Status and function of languages and language varieties (pp. 478-514). Berlin and New York: Walter de Gruyter. Steele, C.M., & Aronson, J. (1995). Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(5), 797-811. Tajfel, H. (1970). Experiments in intergroup discrimination. Scientific American, 223(5), 96- 102. Tajfel, H. (1978). Social categorization, social identity, and social comparison. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 61–76). London: Academic Press. Tajfel, H. & Turner, J.C. (1979). An integrative theory of intergroup conflict. In W.G. Austin, & S. Worchel (Eds.), The social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 33-47). Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. Turner, J.C., & Brown, R.J. (1978). Social status, cognitive alternatives and intergroup relations. In H. Tajfel (Ed.), Differentiation between social groups: Studies in the social psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 201-234). London: Academic Press.
You can also read