Ethnic Differences in Clinical Presentation of Depression in Adult Women
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
R E S E A R C H R E P O R T Ethnic Differences in Clinical Presentation of Depression in Adult Women HECTOR F. MYERS University of California, Los Angeles, and Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science IRA LESSER Harbor–UCLA Medical Center NORMA RODRIGUEZ Pitzer College CONSUELO BINGHAM MIRA Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science WEI-CHIN HWANG University of California, Los Angeles CHRISTINA CAMP DORA ANDERSON Harbor–UCLA Medical Center LUCY ERICKSON Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science MARCY WOHL Harbor–UCLA Medical Center • Hector F. Myers, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA), and Department of Psychiatry, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science; Ira Lesser, Christina Camp, Dora Anderson, and Marcy Wohl, Department of Psychiatry, Harbor–UCLA Medical Center; Norma Rodriguez, Department of Psychology, Pitzer College; Consuelo Bingham Mira and Lucy Erickson, Department of Psychiatry, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science; Wei-Chin Hwang, Department of Psychology, UCLA. Christina Camp is now at the California School of Professional Psychology. This research is supported in part by National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH47913 to the Research Center on the Psychobiology of Ethnicity, Harbor–UCLA Medical Center, and by Na- tional Institutes of Health (NIH) Grant 1 P20 RR11145 to the Research Centers in Minority Institutions Clinical Research Center, Charles R. Drew University of Medicine and Science, and NIH Grant RR00425 to the NIH General Clinical Research Center, Harbor–UCLA Medical Center. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Hector F. Myers, Department of Psychology, UCLA, P. O. Box 951563, Los Angeles, California 90095-1563. E-mail: myers@psych.ucla.edu Cultural Diversity and Ethnic Minority Psychology Copyright 2002 by the Educational Publishing Foundation Vol. 8, No. 2, 138–156 1099-9809/02/$5.00 DOI: 10.1037//1099-9809.8.2.138 138
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 139 This study examined ethnic differences in self-report and interviewer-rated depressive symptoms and estimated the contributions of sociodemographic and psychosocial factors in predicting severity of depression. One hundred twenty-five clinically depressed Afri- can American (n = 46), Caucasian (n = 36), and Latina (n = 43) women were re- cruited. After controlling for differences in socioeconomic status, African American women reported more symptoms of distress and Latinas were rated as significantly more depressed than the other groups. However, these ethnic differences were not moderated by either education or employment. Finally, hierarchical regression analysis indicated that severity of depression was predicted by low education, being single, being Latina, high perceived stress, and feelings of hopelessness. Additional research is needed to validate these results and to investigate their clinical significance. There has been increasing interest in the 2000; Merikangas, 2000). One of the most role ethnicity and culture play in psychiatric consistent findings in this literature is the illness, especially in depression (Lu, Lim, & greater prevalence of major depression Mezzich, 1995). However, relatively few among women in the United States and studies have systematically investigated eth- other developed countries, with most stud- nic differences in the experience and ex- ies reporting a 2:1 female-to-male ratio (Cul- pression of depression and related psycho- bertson, 1997; Kessler, 2000). Various expla- logical distress or the implications that such nations have been offered to account for differences might have for diagnosis, treat- this gender difference, including socioeco- ment, and treatment outcome (Lawson, nomic, biological, personality, differential Hepler, Holladay, & Cuffle, 1994; Myers, exposure to chronic stresses, cognitive and 1993; Zhang & Snowden, 1999). As the U.S. coping styles, and differential patterns of ex- society becomes progressively more multi- pression of distress (Abramson et al., 1999; ethnic, there is an increased need for well- McGrath, Keita, Strickland, & Russo, 1990; designed studies that can help improve di- Nolen-Hoeksema, 1995). However, fewer agnostic accuracy, quality, and effectiveness studies have investigated the role that eth- of treatment and prevention services for nicity might play in accounting for differ- clinically depressed patients from diverse ences in the experience and expression of backgrounds (Neighbors et al., 1992). Thus, depression in general, and specifically in the present study investigated possible eth- women. nic differences in reported depressive symp- All of the evidence to date indicates that toms, as well as in the relative contributions although there are ethnic differences in the of socioeconomic status (SES), sociodemo- expression of depressive symptoms across graphic, and psychosocial risk and protec- ethnic groups, these differences are rela- tive factors in predicting severity of depres- tively minor, and that there is remarkable sion in a multiethnic community sample of consistency in the core features of depres- clinically depressed women. sion across countries and ethnic groups In the United States and worldwide, de- (Ballenger et al., 2001; Weissman et al., pressive disorders account for a significant 1996). Nevertheless, these studies have re- proportion of psychiatric disorders, with ported a greater tendency among depressed women, the poor, young adults, the unem- Caucasians and African Americans to report ployed, the unmarried, and Latinos experi- cognitive-affective symptoms, with African encing a disproportionate burden of mor- Americans reporting more anxiety, anger, bidity (Blazer, Kessler, McGonagle, & and hostility than Caucasians (Fabrega, Mez- Swartz, 1994; Culbertson, 1997; Kessler, zich, & Ulrich, 1988; Raskin, Crook, & Her-
140 MYERS ET AL . man, 1975). There is also evidence suggest- support systems, and psychiatric risk factors ing that African Americans are more likely such as personal and family psychiatric and to report suspiciousness or paranoia than medical history in risk for depression (Got- Caucasians and that this can affect the qual- lib & Hammen, 1992; Kessler & Magee, ity of counselor–client relationship and like- 1993). However, relatively little attention lihood of early treatment termination (Ter- has been given to testing the contribution of rell & Terrell, 1984; Watkins & Terrell, these factors in conferring risk for depres- 1988; Whaley, 1998). However, most of this sion in people of color. Nevertheless, some latter work has been conducted on college research indicates that people of color, es- students and on patients suffering from pecially women of color, face a dispropor- schizophrenic disorders rather than on pa- tionate burden of psychosocial risk from a tients with depression. variety of sources, including chronic and Somewhat less consistent is evidence sug- episodic life events, SES, acculturation, eth- gesting that African Americans and Latinos nic/minority pressures, family, and environ- are also more likely to report somatic com- mental stresses, and that these risks are as- plaints than Caucasians (Compton & Jones, sociated with a disproportionate burden of 1991; Escobar, Rubio-Stipec, Canino, & psychiatric morbidity and mortality (Chis- Karno, 1989; Kirmayer & Young, 1998; holm, 1996; Clark, Anderson, Clark, & Wil- Roberts, 1992; Wohl, Lesser, & Smith, liams, 1999; Crittle, 1996; Hovey, 2000; 1997). This tendency to somatize distress is Rogler, Cortes, & Malgady, 1991; Roysircar- generally associated with lower SES, female Sodowsky & Maestas, 2000; Salgado de gender, older age, monolingual Spanish Snyder, 1987; D. R. Williams, Yu, Jackson, & speakers, immigrants, and low-acculturated Anderson, 1997). However, little is known individuals (Escobar et al., 1989; Guernac- about the contribution that psychosocial cia, Angel, & Worobey, 1989; Kolody, Vega, and psychiatric risk factors make in account- Meinhart, & Bensussen, 1986; Noh, Avison, ing for severity of depression or in shaping & Kaspar, 1992; Swenson, Baxter, Shetterly, the expression of depression in women of Scarbro, & Hamman, 2000). color (Myers, 1993; Nolen-Hoeksema, It is not clear what accounts for these 1995). group differences, although cultural factors Studies have also shown that social un- have been implicated (Ballenger et al., 2001; dermining and low social support are re- Weissman et al., 1996). It is also not clear lated to depression and psychological dis- what effect these differences in the expres- tress in African American women (Gant et sion of depressive symptoms might have on al., 1993) and in Latina immigrants (Sal- diagnostic decisions and on estimates of dis- gado de Snyder, 1987). However, studies ex- ease prevalence. However, Neighbors and amining the role of social support as a mod- colleagues (Neighbors, Jackson, Campbell, erator of the effects of stress on distress have & Williams, 1989), Whaley (1998), and oth- yielded mixed results. Some studies confirm ers have argued that such differences in that social support moderates the stress– symptom expression, however minor, are distress relationship in Latino and African one factor that might account for the appar- American adults and youths (Leadbeater & ent greater risk of psychiatric misdiagnosis Linares, 1992; Padilla, Cervantes, Mal- experienced by African Americans and donado, & Garcia, 1988; Warren, 1997), might complicate treatment decisions (Law- whereas other studies fail to confirm this re- son, 1996). lationship (Crittle, 1996; Snapp, 1992). There is also extensive evidence of the These findings suggest a need to investigate complex interplay of psychosocial risk fac- the interplay of social undermining, stress, tors such as stress and cognitive appraisal and social support in depression among associated with chronic and episodic life women from different ethnic groups. events, childhood adversity, deficits in social Although there is suggestive evidence of
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 141 ethnic differences in the experience and ex- can American and Latina women would re- pression of depression, this research is of port more depressive symptoms and be mixed quality, often fails to distinguish be- rated as more severely depressed than Cau- tween minor syndromal differences and dif- casian women; (b) that the pattern of symp- ferences in core features of the disorder, of- toms would differ such that compared with ten fails to control for demographic or other Caucasian women, African American potentially confounding variables, and relies women would report more hostility, suspi- exclusively on self-report symptom measures ciousness, and somatic complaints (Raskin in nonclinical samples (Myers, 1993). Fur- et al., 1975; Whaley, 1998) and Latinas thermore, it is unclear whether the observed would report more phobic anxiety and so- ethnic differences in symptom expression matic complaints (Aneshensel et al., 1983; are attributable to social class. For instance, Roberts, 1992); and (c) that some of these research by Aneshensel, Clark, and Frerichs differences are expected to be evident even (1983) found greater depressive symptoms after controlling for SES. However, we also among Latinos in comparison with Cauca- expect that SES will moderate the relation- sians and African Americans; however, once ships between ethnicity and severity of de- SES was controlled, the group differences pression, such that the greatest differences disappeared. It is also unclear whether social are observed among the most economically class serves as a moderator, such that within disadvantaged women. Finally, and consis- a given ethnic group, the greatest differ- tent with prevailing evidence, we hypoth- ences in distress and depression are ob- esized (d) that SES, psychosocial, and psy- served among the lower SES, but few or no chiatric risk and protective factors will differences are found among the more af- predict severity of depression independent fluent. It is also of interest to pursue this of ethnicity. moderation effect among ethnically diverse, yet economically similar, populations to in- vestigate whether ethnicity interacts with Method SES under these circumstances (Betancourt & Lopez, 1993). Therefore, research is still Participants needed to investigate whether there are stable and consistent ethnic differences in Clinically depressed African American, Cau- reported symptoms of depression and in se- casian, and Latina women between the ages verity of the disorder in clinically depressed of 18–65 years who sought treatment at one people. of two public psychiatric outpatient clinics The purpose of this study, therefore, was were recruited to participate in this study of to investigate ethnic differences in self- ethnic differences in clinical depression. A reported and interviewer-rated depressive total of 461 women were identified as poten- symptoms, to explore the role SES plays in tially eligible and were screened by tele- explaining such differences, and to estimate phone to determine if they met the follow- the relative contributions of sociodemo- ing study criteria: self-identified as African graphic and psychosocial factors in predict- American, Caucasian, or Latina; had parents ing severity of depression. These questions and grandparents of the same ethnic group; were investigated in a sample of depressed sought treatment for current depression; adult African American, Caucasian, and had no history of mania or psychosis; and Latina women who sought treatment at one reported no alcohol or illicit drug abuse in of two Los Angeles public mental health the past year. One-hundred and thirty-six clinics. We hypothesized, consistent with women met these screening criteria and some previous studies (Brown, Schulberg, & were invited for a formal assessment inter- Madonia, 1996; Malgady & Rogler, 1993; view with a trained, ethnically and linguisti- Raskin et al., 1975), (a) that depressed Afri- cally matched interviewer. Eleven partici-
142 MYERS ET AL . pants were excluded because they met ported a history of psychiatric disorders, criteria for Bipolar I or II, evidenced current 58% (n = 73) reported a family history of psychotic symptoms or had a history of psy- psychiatric disorders, and 50% (n = 62) re- chosis, met criteria for drug or alcohol ported a history of other medical problems. abuse or dependence, or had a physical ill- However, there were no ethnic differences ness that might account for their depression in psychiatric or medical history. (e.g., severe anemia, thyroid dysfunction). Given the extensive amount of time re- Thus, the final sample consisted of 125 quired for the interview and the fact that women (46 African American, 36 Caucasian, data were collected entirely for research and 43 Latina). purposes with no specific benefits to the As seen in Table 1, participants across participants, participants were paid $75 the three ethnic groups were similar in age for the interview and $25 for lab samples, for (M = 42.03 years, SD = 11.03) and reported a total of $100. All participants were treated comparably low annual incomes (M income in accordance with the American Psycho- = $7,934/annum, SD = $5,942). However, logical Association ethical guidelines and Latinas reported significantly lower educa- principles. tion than African Americans and Cauca- sians, F(2, 122) = 45.98, p = .000, and were Measures primarily non-U.S. born (84%, n = 36). Al- though the non-U.S. born Latinas reported DEMOGRAPHIC AND BACKGROUND CHARACTER- an average of 22.8 years of residency in the ISTICS. Age, marital status, years of educa- United States (range = 4–51 years), all spoke tion, occupational status and history, Spanish as their primary language. Latinas monthly income, place of birth, number of also reported consuming significantly less al- years in the United States (if non-U.S. cohol per week, F(2, 122) = 3.51, p = .03, born), primary language, and English profi- than the Caucasian women. ciency (if nonnative English speaker) were As seen in Table 2, Latinas were also obtained from participants. more likely to be employed, 2(2, N = 125) = 18.77, p = .000, although they were earning INTERVIEWER-RATED SYMPTOM SEVERITY MEA- less income and were more likely to be mar- SURES. The Structured Clinical Interview for ried/cohabiting, 2(2, N = 125) = 7.21, p = DSM–IV (SCID–IV; First, Spitzer, Gibbon, & .03, than either the African American or Williams, 1997) was used to assess whether Caucasian women. As a group, the women patients met DSM–IV criteria for major de- reported a substantial burden of psychiatric pression or dysthymia. The majority of the and medical morbidity; 71% (n = 89) re- women enrolled in the study met criteria for TABLE 1 Means and Standard Deviations of Demographic Variables by Ethnic Group African Americans Latinas Caucasians Total (n = 46) (n = 43) (n = 36) (n = 125) Variable M SD M SD M SD M SD Age 43.67 10.84 41.77 11.41 40.25 10.81 42.03 11.03 Education 12.40a 1.82 7.71a, b 3.86 13.00b 2.11 10.96 3.62 Yearly incomea 7.74 5.89 7.88 4.78 9.10 7.12 7.93 5.94 No. of drinks weekly 0.22 0.59 0.09a 0.37 0.39a 0.05 0.23 0.51 No. of cigarettes daily 3.91 7.08 2.48 4.95 3.04 5.27 3.14 5.77 Note. Means in the same row that share the same subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. a Yearly income is reported in thousands.
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 143 TABLE 2 Percentages and Sample Sizes of Demographic Variables by Ethnic Group African Americans Latinas Caucasians Total (n = 46) (n = 43) (n = 36) (n = 125) Variable % n % n % n % n % employed 19.6a 9 62.8a, b 27 30.6b 11 37.6 47 % married/cohabiting 10.9a 5 30.2a, b 13 11.1b 4 17.6 22 % using recreational drugs 4.3 2 4.7 2 11.1 4 6.4 8 % with psychiatric history 71.7 33 69.8 30 72.2 26 71.2 89 % with family psychiatric history 69.6 32 46.5 20 58.3 21 58.4 73 % with history of medical illnesses 56.5 26 46.5 20 44.4 16 49.6 62 Note. Percentages in the same row that share the same subscripts differ significantly at p < .05. major depression (n = 120), and 5 met cri- report symptom measure that uses a 5-point teria for dysthymia. No differences between scale of distress ranging from 0 = not at all to these groups were noted on any of the vari- 4 = extremely. The BSI has been shown to be ables of interest, therefore the analyses were reliable and valid with a variety of popula- run on the entire sample. tions (Hemmings et al., 1999; Skilbeck et al., Severity of depressive symptoms was 1985). The BSI yields three global scores, evaluated using the structured interview but for the purposes of this study, only the guide for the Hamilton Rating Scale for De- Global Severity Index (GSI), which is the pression (HRSD-24; Hamilton, 1960; J. B. W. sum of all items responded to (␣ = .94), was Williams, 1988). This scale includes 24 symp- used as a measure of overall level of psycho- toms of depressive illness rated on either a logical distress. The BSI also yields nine 3-point or 5-point scale that ranges from 0 = symptom subscale scores: somatization, ob- absent to 2 or 4 = severe (␣ = .85; = .78). Only sessive–compulsive, interpersonal sensitivity, patients whose depression was rated as at depression, anxiety, hostility, phobic anxi- least of mild severity (i.e., HRSD > 11) were ety, paranoid ideation, and psychoticism. included in the study. However, to avoid redundancy with the BDI and to reduce testing burden, we omitted SELF-REPORT SYMPTOM SEVERITY MEASURES. the items on the depression subscale from Depressive symptoms were assessed using the subscale comparisons. Subscale scores of the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, the BSI were found to have moderate to 1972), a 21-item self-report measure that as- high reliability in the present sample (al- sesses cognitive, affective, and somatic symp- phas ranged from .59 to .85). toms of depression. Participants were asked to rate the degree of severity of each symp- LAB TESTS. Blood and urine samples were tom on a 4-point scale from 0 = no symptoms taken and assayed to assess participants’ to 3 = severe symptoms. Consistent with previ- overall health and nutritional status and cur- ous research (Hemmings, Reimann, Madri- rent substance use. Lab results were made gal, & Velasquez, 1999; Skilbeck, Acosta, Ya- available to participants, and those with mamoto, & Evans, 1985), the BDI evidenced acute and untreated conditions (e.g., thy- good internal consistency in this sample roid dysfunction, anemia) were excluded (␣ = .83). from the study and referred for treatment. Psychological distress was assessed using the Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI; Dero- PSYCHIATRIC RISK FACTORS. Participants were gatis & Melisaratos, 1983), a 53-item self- queried about their history of use of alcohol
144 MYERS ET AL . and illicit drugs, whether they had ever been (CSQ; Abramson, Metalsky & Alloy, 1993) diagnosed or received treatment for any psy- was used to assess feelings of hopelessness chiatric disorders, whether any member of and helplessness, which is conceptualized as their immediate family had ever been diag- a cognitive style risk factor for depression nosed or treated for a psychiatric disorder, (Abramson et al., 1999; Alloy, Kelly, Mineka, and their history of medical problems. The & Clements, 1990). The CSQ is a 20-item number of drinks in an average week was true-false scale that measures negative ex- computed, as well as the number of ciga- pectations and other dysfunctional cogni- rettes smoked daily. Affirmative responses tions (e.g., “I just don’t get breaks, and on drug use, on personal and family psychi- there’s no reason to believe I will in the fu- atric history, and on medical history were ture” and “My future seems dark to me”). scored 1 and negative responses 0, and sepa- Items were recoded, scored, and summed rate scores were computed for each variable. such that a low score indicates high feelings of hopelessness. This sum score had good PSYCHOSOCIAL RISK FACTORS. Chronic stress reliability (␣ = .89). was assessed with a 21-item revised self- A short three-item Social Undermining report Role Strain Questionnaire (Myers, Scale (SUS; Vinokur & Van Ryn, 1993) was 1985) that assesses on a 4-point scale (1 = not administered to assess stresses that are a problem to 4 = a major problem) the amount caused by members of each participant’s so- of difficulty participants experienced in cial network as an index of interpersonal the past month from a number of sources, stress. Participants were asked to rate, on a including economic, employment, crime, 5-point rating scale (1 = not at all to 5 = a legal problems, discrimination, housing, great deal), the degree to which four impor- transportation, child care, personal con- tant people in their network “act angry or flicts, and illness or accidents. A total unpleasant,” “criticize,” or “make life diffi- chronic stress score was calculated by sum- cult for you.” Responses were summed to ming across all items responded to. Al- form an interpersonal stress score, with though this score was only modestly reliable higher scores reflecting higher levels of in- (␣ = .56), this is most likely attributable to terpersonal stress. The SUS was found to be the fact that strains in one domain (e.g., moderately reliable (␣ = .77). family conflict) need not be related to Finally, social support, which is concep- stresses in other domains (e.g., living in a tualized as a protective resource that may high crime area), yet an overall chronic moderate stress to reduce the severity of de- strain score provides a meaningful index of pression or facilitate recovery (Brugha et al., differences in stress burden that each par- 1987; Brugha et al., 1990; McLeod, Kessler, ticipant carries. & Landis, 1992), was assessed with a short Perceived stress was assessed with the five-item version of the Social Support Ques- 14-item Perceived Stress Scale (Cohen, Kar- tionnaire (Sarason, Levine, Basham, & Sara- marck & Mermelstein, 1983) that measures son, 1983). The measure asks participants to the degree to which life events are evaluated identify and rate the four most important as uncontrollable, emotionally overloading, people in their lives on the degree to which and unpredictable (e.g., “In the last month, they provide advice, emotional support, and how often have you felt nervous or stressed” instrumental support. Ratings are made on and “In the last month, how often have you a 5-point scale from 1 = not at all to 5 = a found that you could not cope with all the great deal. An overall sum score was calcu- things you had to do”). Respondents rated lated (␣ = .91), with higher scores reflecting each item on a 5-point scale from 0 = never to higher levels of support. 4 = very often, and a sum score was calculated that was moderately reliable in this sample Procedure (␣ = .73). At the interview, trained and ethnically and The Cognitive Style Questionnaire linguistically matched female interviewers
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 145 described the purpose of the study and An a priori alpha level of .05 was used for all study procedures and requirements, written of the statistical analyses reported. consent to participate was obtained, biologi- Because Latinas were significantly less cal samples were collected, the diagnostic educated but were more likely to be em- interview and interviewer-rated assessments ployed and married or cohabiting than Af- were conducted, and self-report measures rican American and Caucasian women, were administered. All interviewers were these variables were treated as covariates in trained and supervised in the administration subsequent analyses. These covariates were of the diagnostic interviews and in rating the dichotomized into the following values: edu- range and severity of depressive symptoms cation: 12 years by one of the coauthors (Ira Lesser), with of education; employment status: full/part periodic reviews of diagnostic procedures time or unemployed; and marital status: and consultation on the more difficult cases. married/cohabiting or single/unattached. All of the instruments were originally de- Although Latinas also consumed signifi- veloped in English and subsequently trans- cantly less alcohol than the other women, lated into Spanish by a certified translator. the quantities of alcohol were so small and The Spanish version of each instrument was unlikely to have a significant role in depres- then back-translated by an English/Spanish sion that this variable was omitted from sub- bilingual individual familiar with psychologi- sequent analyses to save degrees of freedom. cal research, and discrepancies between the English and Spanish versions of the protocol were resolved, thus “decentering” the origi- nal English version (Brislin, 1993). Thirty- Differences in Clinical Status six (84%) of the Latina women chose to be interviewed in Spanish, and the remaining 7 A MANCOVA controlling for education, (16%) were interviewed in English. Both the employment, and marital status was run test- English and Spanish protocols were pre- ing for group differences on severity of de- tested on a subsample of patients and non- pression as measured by the HRSD-24, de- patients, and minor additional changes were pressive symptoms as measured by the BDI, made to improve the clarity and compre- and psychological distress as measured by hensibility of the measures. the BSI–GSI. As shown in Table 3, there was a signifi- cant main effect of ethnicity on clinical sta- tus, Wilks’s lambda F(6, 234) = 4.74, p = .000. Results Between-subjects tests indicated that there were significant group differences on inter- A series of univariate analyses tested for eth- viewer-rated severity of depression (HRSD- nic group differences at entry into treat- 24), F(2, 119) = 10.69, p = .000, and on self- ment on demographic characteristics. Sub- reports of psychological distress (GSI), sequently, we used a series of multivariate F(2,119) = 3.04, p = .05. Pairwise compari- analyses of covariance (MANCOVA), con- sons indicated that Latinas were rated as trolling for education, employment, and more severely depressed than both African marital status to test for ethnic differences in American (p = .000) and Caucasian (p = clinical status, symptom expression, and psy- .001) women and that African American chosocial factors. Next, we conducted a hi- women reported significantly more psycho- erarchical regression analysis to investigate logical distress than Caucasian women (p = whether SES moderates the observed ethnic .05) but not more than Latinas. There was differences in severity of depression. Finally, also a strong trend (p = .06) on the BDI, with we ran a hierarchical regression analysis to Latinas reporting more symptoms of depres- identify which of the factors studied were sion than Caucasian women (p = .05) but the best predictors of severity of depression. not more than African American women.
146 MYERS ET AL . TABLE 3 Adjusted Means and Standard Errors of Clinical Status at Entry Into Treatment by Ethnic Group African Americans Latinas Caucasians Total (n = 46) (n = 43) (n = 36) (n = 125) Variable M SE M SE M SE M SE Interviewer-rated HRSD 23.34a 1.31 32.79a, b 1.50 24.40b 1.51 26.84 .77 Self-report BDI total 31.01 1.37 34.12 1.57 28.35 1.58 31.16 .80 BSI Global Score GSI 58.73a 3.71 56.36 4.26 45.45a 4.30 53.51 2.18 Note. Means that share the same subscript differ at p < .05. HRSD = Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression; BDI = Beck Depression Inventory; BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory; GSI = Global Severity Index. Differences in Symptom Expression F(2, 119) = 4.54, p = .01. Pairwise compari- sons indicated that African American To test the hypothesis of ethnic differences women reported more paranoid symptoms in symptom expression, we included all of (p = .001) than Caucasian women, and both the BSI subscales except the depression sub- African American women (p = .02) and Lati- scale in a MANCOVA controlling for educa- nas (p = .05) reported significantly more so- tion, employment, and marital status. As matic symptoms than Caucasian women. shown in Table 4, a significant overall differ- Strong trends (p = .06) were also observed ence was obtained, Wilks’s lambda F(16, on paranoid symptoms and anxiety, with 224) = 2.32, p = .003; however, only partial Latinas reporting more of these symptoms support for this hypothesis was obtained. than Caucasian women. However, the ex- More specifically, between-subjects tests in- pected differences on symptoms of hostil- dicated that significant differences were ity (African American > Caucasian) and only apparent on paranoid symptoms, F(2, phobic anxiety (Latina > Caucasian) were 119) = 7.57, p = .001, and on somatization, not observed. TABLE 4 Adjusted Means and Standard Errors of Symptom Expression of Depression at Treatment Entry by Ethnic Group African Americans Latinas Caucasians Total (n = 46) (n = 43) (n = 36) (n = 125) Brief Symptom Inventory M SE M SE M SE M SE Obsessive–compulsive 9.37 .73 9.73 .83 8.77 .84 9.29 .43 Interpersonal sensitivity 5.27 .49 5.75 .57 4.80 .57 5.42 .29 Anxiety 7.32 .64 8.93 .73 6.28 .74 7.51 .37 Hostility 4.93 .54 3.26 .62 4.01 .62 4.06 .32 Phobic anxiety 5.51 .64 5.34 .74 4.09 .75 4.98 .38 Paranoid ideation 6.20a .49 5.40 .56 3.38a .57 4.99 .29 Psychotic 5.75 .46 4.59 .53 4.25 .54 4.86 .27 Somatization 7.10a .67 7.15b .78 4.31a, b .77 6.19 .43 Note. Means that share same subscript differ at p < .05.
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 147 Differences on Psychosocial Factors moderates the observed ethnic differences in severity of depression (HRSD-24). A MANCOVA controlling for years of edu- In each analysis, education and employ- cation, employment, and marital status were ment served as the indicators of SES, and run testing for ethnic differences on feelings following the procedures suggested by of hopelessness, chronic stress, perceived Baron and Kenny (1986), variables were en- stress, social support, and social undermin- tered in sets and in three steps. In Step 1, ing. As shown in Table 5, results indicated education and employment were entered. that there was a significant overall ethnic dif- In Step 2, ethnicity was dummy coded into ference, Wilks’s lambda F(10, 230) = 1.85, Ethnic 1 (African American compared with p = .05; however, between-subjects tests indi- Caucasian) and Ethnic 2 (African American cated that only one significant group differ- compared with Latina) and entered. African ence on social support was obtained, F(2, Americans were chosen as the referent 119) = 3.01, p = .05. African American group because of its larger sample size, women reported receiving more social sup- which would yield greater stability in the re- port than Caucasian women. There was also sults. In Step 3, four interaction terms were a trend on social undermining (p = .08), entered: Education × Ethnic 1, Education × with Latinas reporting less social undermin- Ethnic 2, Employment × Ethnic 1, and Em- ing than Caucasian women. No differences ployment × Ethnic 2. on feelings of hopelessness, chronic stress, or perceived stress were obtained. Moderation of Ethnicity on Severity of Depression by SES SES as a Moderator of Ethnic Differences As seen in Table 6, the regression equation Studies of ethnic differences in mental testing SES as a moderator on severity of health often fail to adequately account for depression accounted for 20% of the vari- the possible effects of SES. It is very likely, ance, F(8, 116) = 4.75, p = .000. In Step 1, for example, that SES may exert its effects education was significantly and negatively on clinical outcomes either independent of related to the severity of depression, ac- ethnicity or as a moderator of ethnicity. In counting for 16% of the total variance ex- the previous analyses, the independent ef- plained, Fchange(2, 122) = 11.84, p = .000. fects of SES were statistically controlled be- This effect indicated that low education con- fore testing for ethnic differences. In the tributes significantly to the severity of de- next analyses, we investigate whether SES pression. In Step 2, although the step was TABLE 5 Adjusted Means and Standard Errors of Psychosocial Factors at Entry Into Treatment by Ethnic Group African Americans Latinas Caucasians Total (n = 46) (n = 43) (n = 36) (n = 125) Variable M SE M SE M SE M SE Hopelessness 28.32 0.89 26.81 1.02 28.64 1.03 27.92 0.52 Chronic stress 41.70 1.05 39.86 1.20 39.78 1.21 40.44 0.61 Perceived stress 37.42 0.97 39.86 1.11 38.64 1.12 38.64 0.57 Social support 74.86a 2.43 70.24 2.79 66.10a 2.81 70.40 1.43 Social undermining 23.46 1.18 20.93 1.35 25.63 1.36 23.34 0.69 Note. Means that share the same subscript differ at p < .05.
148 MYERS ET AL . TABLE 6 Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Moderation of Ethnicity on Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression by Socioeconomic Status (N = 125) Step Variable B SE B  R2 change F change 1 Socioeconomic status 0.16 11.84*** Education −3.80 0.82 −0.41*** Employment −0.35 1.68 −0.02 2 Ethnicity 0.04 2.78 Ethnic 1 1.68 1.90 0.08 Ethnic 2 5.38 2.29 0.28* 3 Education & Employment × Ethnicity Interaction Terms 0.05 1.81 Education × Ethnic 1 −1.26 5.80 −0.14 Education × Ethnic 2 −4.95 3.32 −0.53 Employment × Ethnic 1 0.44 1.24 0.05 Employment × Ethnic 2 2.89 1.71 0.31 Note. R2 = 0.25 (adjusted R2 = 0.20), F(8, 116) = 4.75, p = .000. Ethnic 1 was coded 0 = African American, 1 = Caucasian, and Ethnic 2 was coded 0 = African American, 1 = Latina. *p < .05. ***p < .001. not significant, ethnicity made a marginal moderates stress in predicting severity of additional contribution, Fchange(2, 120) = depression. 2.78, p = .07, indicating that being Latina as As shown in Table 7, the regression opposed to African American predicted equation predicting severity of depression more severe depression. In Step 3, none of accounted for 32% of the variance, F(11, the interaction terms were significant, which 113) = 6.21, p = .000. In Step 1, low educa- indicates that neither education nor em- tion and being single were significant inde- ployment moderated ethnicity in account- pendent predictors of severity of depression, ing for differences on severity of depression. Fchange = 9.43, p = .000, and the entire step accounted for 19% of the total variance. In Step 2, ethnicity was significantly related to Predictors of Severity of Depression depression and accounted for an additional We conducted a hierarchical regression 5% of the variance, Fchange = 3.70, p = .03. analysis to determine which psychosocial This effect indicated that after controlling factors predict severity of depression as in- for differences in demographic characteris- dexed by HRSD-24 scores and controlling tics, Latinas were rated as more severely de- for SES, ethnicity, and psychiatric risk fac- pressed than African Americans. Personal tors. Variables were entered in sets and in and family psychiatric history in Step 3 was seven steps: Education, marital status, and not significant. In Step 4, perceived stress employment were entered in Step 1; ethnic- was positively and significantly associated ity was entered in Step 2 as dummy variables with severity of depression, Fchange = 11.10, Ethnic 1 (African American compared with p = .001, and accounted for an additional Caucasian) and Ethnic 2 (African American 6% of the variance. In Step 5, after control- compared with Latina); personal and family ling for all previous steps, hopelessness psychiatric history were entered in Step 3; accounted for an additional 7% of perceived stress was entered in Step 4; hope- the variance, Fchange = 4.77, p = .03, indicat- lessness as an index of psychological vulner- ing that the greater the feelings of hope- ability was entered in Step 5; social support lessness, the greater the severity of de- was entered in Step 6; and a Perceived Stress pression. Contrary to expectations, however, × Social Support interaction term was en- social support in Step 6 and the Perceived tered in Step 7 to test whether social support Stress × Social Support interaction term in
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 149 TABLE 7 Hierarchical Regression Analysis of Severity of Depression With Background Characteristics, Ethnicity, Psychiatric History, Perceived Stress, Hopelessness, and Social Support as Predictors (N = 125) Step Variable B SE B  R2 change F change 1 Background characteristics 0.19 9.43*** Marital status −4.07 2.03 −0.17* Employment 0.03 1.68 0.00 Education −4.01 0.82 −0.43*** 2 Ethnicity 0.05 3.70* Ethnic 1 1.66 1.87 0.08 Ethnic 2 6.16 2.26 0.32** 3 Psychiatric history 0.03 1.98 Participant psychiatric history −1.12 1.73 −0.06 Family psychiatric history 3.14 1.58 0.17* 4 Perceived stress 0.38 0.11 0.27 0.06 11.10** 5 Hopelessness −0.16 0.07 −0.20 0.03 4.77* 6 Social support −1.27 0.73 −0.14 0.02 2.97 7 Perceived Stress × Social Support interaction term 4.64 4.11 0.50 0.01 1.28 Note. R2 = 0.38 (adjusted R2 = 0.32), F(11, 113) = 6.21, p < .001. Ethnic 1 was coded 0 = African American, 1 = Caucasian and Ethnic 2 was coded 0 = African American, 1 = Latina. *p < .05. **p < .01. ***p < .001. Step 7 were unrelated to severity of depres- sian counterparts. It is also worth noting, sion, which indicated that social support however, that no substantive differences in had no direct protective effect and did not core symptoms of the disorder were ob- moderate stress in predicting severity of served, which is consistent with previous depression. findings of no differences in core features of depression across ethnic or national groups (Ballenger et al., 2001). Discussion Although we hypothesized that African American and Latina women would be rated This study was conducted to investigate eth- as more severely depressed than Caucasian nic differences in self-report and inter- women, our results indicated that only Lati- viewer-rated depressive symptoms, to ex- nas were rated as more severely depressed, plore the role SES plays in explaining such after controlling for differences in educa- differences, and to estimate the relative con- tion, employment, and marital status. The tributions of sociodemographic and psycho- greater severity ratings of Latinas are consis- social factors in predicting severity of de- tent with previous evidence of more severe pression in a multiethnic sample of clinically depression among less-educated, immi- depressed adult women. Results from this grant, and monolingual Spanish speakers study provided partial support for previous (Malgady & Rogler, 1993; Skilbeck et al., findings that ethnic groups differ in the ex- 1985; Swenson et al., 2000). We considered pression of clinical depression assessed the possibility that this finding was an arti- through self-report and interviewer-rated fact of measurement, but this is unlikely be- measures (Ballenger et al., 2001; Fa- cause of the congruence between self-report brega et al., 1988; Neff, 1984; Raskin et al., symptoms of distress and depression and the 1975; Wohl et al., 1997). As expected, Lati- interviewer ratings of symptom severity. nas and African American women reported Thus, perhaps, the Latinas were more se- more somatic complaints than their Cauca- verely depressed because of greater willing-
150 MYERS ET AL . ness to report symptoms of distress or be- differences in education, employment, and cause of delay in seeking professional care marital status, only the comparison between compared with the African American and the Latinas and Caucasian women ap- Caucasian women. However, it is also pos- proached significance (p = .06). Neverthe- sible that this difference could be due, at less, the self-report data in conjunction with least in part, to a tendency of the clinical the interviewer ratings suggest that the Lati- interviewer to rate the Latinas more se- nas, in particular, experience greater levels verely. A report by Malgady and Costantino of depressive symptomatology compared (1998) indicated that Latino clinicians rated with the other two groups. This higher level psychiatric symptoms in bilingual Hispanics of depressive symptoms might be attributed more severely than Caucasian clinicians, and to the additional sociocultural burdens this was especially evident for bilingual and (e.g., acculturation) they face, as well as monolingual Spanish interviewers. Unfortu- their postponing seeking professional men- nately, this hypothesis could not be tested in tal health services. It is worth reinforcing, this study because only trained, bilingual– however, that this is not a difference in the bicultural Latinas interviewed the Latina core symptoms of depression but mainly a patients, and thus, no comparisons by eth- difference in symptom reporting. nicity of interviewer could be made. Never- Consistent with our expectations and theless, these results await replication in controlling for differences in education, em- future studies, and the competing hypoth- ployment, and marital status, depressed eses of greater symptom reporting versus de- African American women also reported layed treatment seeking versus ethnicity of greater psychological distress than Cauca- the interviewer merit further investigation. sian women. These results confirm previous Epidemiological studies indicate lower reports from community surveys (D. R. rates of depression among recent Latino im- Brown & Gary, 1985; Dressler & Badger, migrants compared with their U.S.-born 1985) and clinical studies (C. Brown et al., counterparts (Vega, Kolody, Aguilar- 1996; Raskin et al., 1975) that indicate a ten- Graxiola, & Catalano, 1998). Unfortunately, dency for African American women to re- because our Latina sample primarily con- port more psychological distress than Cau- sisted of immigrants, we were not able to casian women. It is interesting to note, compare them with their U.S.-born counter- however, that the African American women parts. However, it is important to note that rated themselves as more psychologically although low in acculturation (i.e., 84% distressed than the ratings given by the in- were monolingual Spanish speakers), the terviewers would suggest. This apparent dis- Latina women in our sample were not re- crepancy may reflect the fact that the range cent immigrants, with an average of 23 years of symptoms available on the self-report of residency in the United States. Thus, as measure is greater and more diverse than reported in other studies, pressures of accul- the symptoms that are the focus of attention turation, persistent socioeconomic disadvan- on the interviewer assessment of severity of tage in a country of considerable wealth, ex- depression. Therefore, their overall level of posure to anti-immigrant and racial distress may be more related to the fact that prejudice, and other noxious social pro- they are slightly older, more likely to be un- cesses may accumulate to erode whatever employed, less likely to be married or cohab- protections these immigrants bring and con- iting, somewhat more likely to have a psychi- tribute to increased psychiatric vulnerability atric history, and to carry a higher burden of over time (Vega et al., 1998). medical morbidity than the Caucasian and Although the African American and Latina women, rather than any substantive Latina women reported more depressive difference in core symptoms of depression. symptoms in the self-report measures than As shown in previous research (Terrell & the Caucasian women, after controlling for Terrell, 1984; Whaley, 1998), our results also
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 151 indicated that African American women re- contrary to previous reports that found ported more distrust than Caucasian women higher rates of somatic complaints among and that these differences were not related Latinos (Compton & Jones, 1991; Golding, to SES. This greater cultural mistrust on the Aneshensel, & Hough, 1991; Roberts, 1992; part of African Americans has been shown Sylva, 1997). This finding was also somewhat to influence their interactions with health unexpected given that the Latinas were sig- and mental health care providers (Terrell & nificantly less educated and low accultur- Terrell, 1984) and may increase the risk for ated. Nevertheless, the elevated levels of so- clinicians to overinterpret the psychiatric matic complaints by the African American significance of these symptoms rather than women may simply be reflective of their seeing them as nonpathological idioms of overall greater psychological distress. distress (Whaley, 1998). Contrary to expectations, there were no Although not significant, Latinas also re- differences in hostility between African ported more mistrust than Caucasian American women and the other women. women, and along with the African Ameri- Previous studies reporting these differences can women’s reported levels of mistrust were based on more severely depressed hos- comparable with the adult psychiatric out- pitalized patients (Raskin et al., 1975) or on patient sample used in the norming of the samples that included men (Fabrega et al., BSI (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983). The 1988) rather than on moderately depressed Caucasian women, in contrast, reported lev- women. Similarly, the expected differences els of mistrust more comparable with the on phobic anxiety between the Latina and adult nonpatient norming sample of the the Caucasian women were not observed BSI. It is reasonable to expect greater mis- (Karno et al., 1987). Additional research is trust among residents in high-stress, high- needed to determine whether ethnic differ- crime communities, such as those who par- ences in phobic anxiety are likely to be mod- ticipated in this study. What is interesting, erated by severity of depression, gender, or however, is that the same level of mistrust age. was not evident in the Caucasian women in As suggested by several authors (Betan- our sample. This difference in distrust is not court & Lopez , 1993; Myers, 1993; Williams attributable to SES and suggests that it is et al., 1997), it is important to disentangle likely to be culturally mediated, as suggested the effects of ethnicity from SES, so that ob- by Watkins and Terrel (1988) and Whaley served group differences are not errone- (1998). Additional exploration is warranted ously attributed to ethnicity. In the present to examine whether higher levels of mistrust study, SES, as indexed by education, employ- is a culturally congruent way of expressing ment, and marital status, either was statisti- distress among women of color and whether cally controlled or its independent effects lower social class exacerbates these feelings and its role as a possible moderator of eth- of mistrust. nicity were tested. The results indicated that The expected greater tendency toward there were ethnic differences in symptom expressing somatic symptoms of distress reporting after controlling for differences among women of color (C. Brown et al., on SES and that SES did not moderate eth- 1996; Escobar et al., 1989; Kolody et al., nicity in accounting for differences in sever- 1986; Noh et al., 1992) was confirmed, with ity of depression. Therefore, other sociocul- both the African American and Latina tural factors that were not examined in women reporting significantly more somatic these analyses may be implicated in account- complaints than the Caucasian women, after ing for the group differences observed. controlling for differences in SES. Although Finally, we also investigated what role not statistically significant, the African psychosocial factors play in accounting for American women reported more somatic differences in severity of depression. We ini- complaints than the Latina women, which is tially investigated ethnic group differences
152 MYERS ET AL . in the psychosocial factors, controlling for depression cannot be determined from SES. These initial comparisons yielded eth- these data. Subsequent analyses will explore nic differences only on social support, with this question using independent ratings of African American women reporting more chronic and episodic stress as predictors of social support than Caucasian women. Nev- severity of depression. ertheless, when these psychosocial factors Finally, these results are based only on were entered into a regression equation, se- low-income and working-class women seek- verity of depression was associated with feel- ing psychiatric care at public outpatient clin- ings of hopelessness and higher perceived ics. It would be useful to determine whether stress along with low education, being these results are replicated with comparable single, and being Latina in comparison with samples of men and with men and women of being African American. The significance of higher SES. It would also be useful to deter- the strong association of feelings of hope- mine whether these minor syndromal differ- lessness and perceived stress with depression ences observed at entry into treatment are should not be overinterpreted because the associated with differences in diagnostic de- tendency to overrate life difficulties is con- cisions or predict treatment received, treat- founded with severity of depression (Gott- ment response, and treatment outcome over lieb & Hammen, 1992), which precludes dis- time. All of the participants were in treat- entangling the direction of the stress–de- ment, and subsequent articles will address pression relationship. this issue. The results also indicated that social sup- port was not an independent predictor of depression, nor did it serve as a moderator References of stress. These findings are consistent with previous studies that failed to support the Abramson, L. Y., Alloy, L. B., Hogan, M. E., hypothesized moderating effect of social Whitehouse, W. G., Donovan, P., Rose, D. J., support (Crittle, 1996; Snapp, 1992). How- et al. (1999). Cognitive vulnerability to de- ever, these results are not entirely surprising pression: Theory and evidence. Journal of Cog- because all of the women are clinically de- nitive Psychotherapy, 13, 5–20. pressed. Therefore, whatever protective or Abramsom, L. Y., Metalsky, G. I., & Alloy, L. B. moderating effect their social resources (1993). Hopelessness. In C. G. Costello (Ed.), might have provided has already proven Symptoms of depression (pp. 181–205). New inadequate. York: Wiley. In summary, the results of this study pro- Alloy, L. B., Kelly, K. A., Mineka, S., & Clements, vide additional, although somewhat limited C. M. (1990). Comorbidity of anxiety and de- support, of ethnic differences in the expres- pressive disorders: A helplessness–hopeless- sion of depression among clinically de- ness perspective. In J. D. Maser & C. R. Clon- pressed women but not in the core symp- inger (Eds.), Comorbidity of mood and anxiety disorders (pp. 499–543). Washington, DC: toms of the disorder. These differences are American Psychiatric Press. not attributable to differences in SES and Aneshensel, C. S., Clark, V. A., & Frerichs, R. R. need to be replicated in larger and more (1983). Race, ethnicity and depression: A socioeconomically diverse samples. The re- confirmatory analysis. Journal of Personality sults also suggest the need to explore pos- and Social Psychology, 44, 385–398. sible ethnocultural and psychosocial expla- Ballenger, J. C., Davidson, J. R., Lecrubier, Y., nations for these differences. Our results Nutt, D. J., Kirmayer, L. J., Lepine, J. P., et al. also confirm that depressed women, irre- (2001). Consensus statement on transcultural spective of their ethnic background, experi- issues in depression and anxiety from the In- ence high chronic strain. However, whether ternational Consensus Group on Depression this greater stress burden is a contributor to and Anxiety. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, their depression or a consequence of their 62(Suppl. 13), 47–55.
ETHNIC DIFFERENCES IN DEPRESSION 153 Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The mod- Journal of Health and Social Behaviors, 24, erator–mediator variable distinction in social 385–396. psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, Compton, A. B., & Jones, F. D. (1991). Clinical and statistical considerations. Journal of Per- features of young adult Hispanic psychiatric sonality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. in-patients: The so-called “Puerto Rican syn- Beck, A. T. (1972). Measuring depression: The drome.” Military Medicine, 156, 351–354. depression inventory. In T. A. Williams, M. Crittle, M. C. (1996). Stress, social support, and M. Katz, & J. A. Shields (Eds.), Recent advances well-being in midlife Black and White in the psychobiology of the depressive illnesses (pp. women. Dissertation Abstracts International B, 299–302). Washington, DC: U.S. Govern- 57(6-B), 4088. ment Printing Office. Culbertson, F. M. (1997). Depression and gen- Betancourt, H., & Lopez, S. R. (1993). The study der: An international review. American Psy- of culture, ethnicity, and race in American chologist, 52, 25–31. psychology. American Psychologist, 48, Derogatis, L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The 629–637. Brief Symptom Inventory: An introductory Blazer, D. G., Kessler, R. C., McGonagle, K. A., & report. Psychological Medicine, 13, 595–605. Swartz, M. S. (1994). The presence and dis- Dressler, W. W., & Badger, L. W. (1985). Epide- tribution of major depression in a national miology of depressive symptoms in Black community sample: The National Cormor- communities: A comparative analysis. Journal bidity Survey. American Journal of Psychiatry, of Nervous and Mental Disorders, 173, 212–220. 151, 979–986. Escobar, J. I., Rubio-Stipec, M., Canino, G., & Brislin, R. (1993). Understanding culture’s influence Karno, M. (1989). Somatic Symptom Index on behavior. San Diego, CA: Harcourt Brace (SSI): A new and abridged somatization con- Jovanovich. struct: Prevalence and epidemiological corre- Brown, C., Schulberg, H. C., & Madonia, M. J. lates in two large community samples. Journal (1996). Clinical presentations of major de- of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 140–146. pression by African Americans and Whites in Fabrega, H., Mezzich, J., & Ulrich, R. F. (1988). primary medical care practice. Journal of Af- African American–White differences in psy- fective Disorders, 41, 181–191. chopathology in an urban psychiatric popu- Brown, D. R., & Gary, L. E. (1985). Predictors of lation. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29, 285–297. depressive symptoms among unemployed First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., & Wil- Black adults. Journal of Sociology and Social Wel- liams, J. B. W. (1997). Structured Clinical Inter- fare, 12, 736–754. view for DSM–IV Axis I Disorders—Clinician Ver- Brugha, T. S., Bebbington, P. E., MacCarthy, B., sion (SCID–CV). Washington, DC: American Potter, J., Sturt, E., & Wykes, T. (1987). Social Psychiatric Press. networks, social support and the type of de- Gant, L. M., Nagda, B. A., Brabson, H. H., Ja- pressive illness. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, yaratne, S., Chess, W. A., & Singh, A. (1993). 76, 664–673. Effects of social support and undermining on Brugha, T. S., Bebbington, P. E., MacCarthy, B., African American worker’s perceptions of co- Sturt, E., Wykes, T., & Potter, J. (1990). Gen- worker and supervisor relationships and psy- der, social support and recovery from depres- chological well-being. Social Work, 38, 158– sive disorders: A prospective clinical study. 164. Psychological Medicine, 20, 147–156. Golding, J. M., Aneshensel, C. S., & Hough, R. L. Chisholm, J. F. (1996). Mental health issues in (1991). Responses to depression scale items African-American women. Annals of the New among Mexican-Americans and non-His- York Academy of Sciences, 789, 161–179. panic Euro-Americans. Journal of Clinical Psy- Clark, R., Anderson, N. B., Clark, V. R., & Wil- chiatry, 47, 161–171. liams, D. R. (1999). Racism as a stressor for Gotlib, I. H., & Hammen, C. L. (1992). Psychologi- African Americans: A biopsychosocial model. cal aspects of depression: Toward a cognitive–in- American Psychologist, 54, 805–816. terpersonal integration. Chichester, UK: Wiley. Cohen, S., Karmarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. Guernaccia, P. J., Angel, R., & Worobey, J. L. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. (1989). The factor structure of the CES-D in
You can also read