Emotional Intelligence in Sport: Theoretical Linkages and Preliminary Empirical Relationships from Basketball
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Emotional Intelligence in Sport: Theoretical Linkages and Preliminary Empirical Relationships from Basketball *Con Stough1, Mathilde Clements1,2 and Lena Wallish1 1 Swinburne University of technology 2 Mental Edge Consulting *To whom correspondence should be addressed: Brain Sciences Institute Swinburne University PO Box 218 Hawthorn Victoria, 3122 AUSTRALIA 1
Recently there has been suggestions from within sports psychology (e.g., Meyer & Fletcher, 2007) that there are substantial similarities in the use of psychological tools and programs in the corporate and sporting worlds. Therefore it is somewhat surprising that despite the growing body of research supporting emotional intelligence (EI) as an important tool for identifying superior performance levels within the workplace, it is yet to be the subject of rigorous research within other performance arenas such as the sporting environment. The aim of this chapter is to explore the potential relationship between EI and sport, with emphasis on the potential role of EI in separating athletes on the basis of their type of sport. The chapter starts with a discussion of earlier conceptions of emotions in sport, progresses to discuss the construct of emotional intelligence and then provides some empirical data assessing the utility of at least one application of emotional intelligence to the elite sporting arena. Throughout the chapter we propose theoretical linkages between EI and elite sport variables which should be the focus of future empirical research. Emotions in Sports Sports psychologists and professional athletes have started to evaluate the linkages between emotion and competitive sporting performance, and in particular how moderating and appropriately expressing the experience of emotions can facilitate performance (Vallerand, 1983). To date a majority of research into the impact of emotion on sporting performance has been focused on the control of the physical 2
manifestation of emotions such as rage, frustration and how they impact on performance rather than on the cognitive management of the emotions that caused the emotional display (Vallerand, 1983). Only recently has research broadened to look at the more cognitive side of emotion, with researchers such as Schachter (1964), Lazarus (1966) and Weiner (1981) all proposing cognitive theories, in which both arousal and cognitions are required in the experience of emotion. While it is generally well acknowledged that emotions play an important role in the sporting arena (D’Urso, Andreina & Robazza, 2002), the exact nature of their role in sporting performance is still very under- researched. It has been argued that not only is the expression of emotion highly prevalent on the sporting field but an essential aspect of performance in all sports (Vallerand, 1983). For example, the inability to appropriately manage emotions experienced in competitive situations may lead to such things as an inappropriate outburst of rage or aggression and can often lead athletes to be penalized or excluded from competing. While those who are able to effectively manage their emotions can channel their emotions into the production of motivation and drive. It is therefore important that athletes learn how to recognize these emotions, express them appropriately and manage them effectively (Botterill & Brown, 2002). While there has been difficulty providing a clear and precise definition of emotion (Lazarus, 1991a), researchers have generally established three main components of emotion: subjective experience, physiological changes, and observable behaviour (Young, 1973), and in each individual study the researcher uses a variation of this basic theory, identifying different guidelines for what constitutes each of the three stages. 3
Many of the current theories on the role of emotion in sport are limited to one aspect of emotion, such as optimal levels of arousal or balance between positive and negative emotions, which means that while each individual theory adds to our understanding, there is no one particular model that can be used to explain the complete relationship between the full range of our emotions and sporting performance (D’Urso, Petrosso & Robazza, 2002). There has been an acknowledgement that there is a need for a model that integrates the important contributions from each of the major theories (Crocker & Graham, 1995). Recently Hanin (2000) argued that there was a lack of research investigating the role of emotion in sport. With most of the research being inclined to focus on anxiety- performance relationships, what little research that has been done in this area has tended to have a negative emotion bias. Yet recently there has been a growing movement amongst researchers to examine the role of emotions in sporting performance (Hanin, 2000). Recent research by Hanin and Stambulova (2002) has identified the importance in distinguishing a specific set of emotion content that is optimal or dysfunctional for an athlete’s performance. The research has suggested that due to the dynamic nature of emotional content that it would be useful to isolate temporal patterns of emotions throughout a particular competition or several competitions so as to plan psychological interventions and strategies for performance improvement (Hanin & Stambulova, 2002). 4
Based on Hanin’s Individual Zones of Optimal Functioning (IZOF) model, it has been proposed that the function of emotions in the sporting arena be studied through five basic dimensions; form, content, intensity, time and context (Crust, 2002). Acknowledging that a wide range of emotions, other than anxiety, characterize sporting experience, Hanin’s model (2000) suggests that a range of positive and negative emotions can both facilitate, as well as inhibit, performance. The model suggests that positive and negative emotions may exert beneficial or detrimental effects depending on their idiosyncratic meaning and intensity. That is, a specific emotion may be beneficial for one athlete but may be detrimental for another, depending on the meaning the individual attaches to that emotion. Importantly the IZOF model suggests that different intensities of an emotion may result in improved or impaired performance in the same athlete. Each athlete differs on the interaction of affect hedonic tone (positive or negative) and functional impact (facilitating or inhibiting) (Hanin, 2000). Because of the interactive effects of emotions the functional impact of emotions upon performance can be best explained by resource-matching (Robazza & Bortoli, 2002). That is, facilitating-positive and –negative emotions will reflect the availability of resources and their effective recruitment and use. To sustain mental and physical effort in achieving goals, facilitating-positive emotions would help the athlete to produce energy and organize functions. Conversely, facilitating-negative emotions would result in energy production than utilization (Robazza & Bortoli, 2002). In keeping with this concept, inhibiting-positive and –negative emotions will reflect a lack or loss of resources or the inefficient recruitment and utilization of them. Inhibiting- 5
positive emotions will reflect a decreased effort or energy loss, whereas inhibiting- negative emotions will result in an inadequate energy production and utilization (Robazza & Bortoli, 2002). The model assumes that the total impact of emotion upon performance can be predicted on the basis of interactive rather than separate effects of energy mobilization and energy utilization functions (Hanin, 2000). Mayer and Salovey’s Ability Model of Emotional Intelligence Salovey and Mayer first coined the term ‘emotional intelligence’ (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). Formulating the term as a challenge to intelligence theorists, who have historically considered arousal of affect as disorganizing cognitive activity, Salovey and Mayer (1990) described Emotional Intelligence as a form of social intelligence. Consistent with earlier research on social intelligence (Ford & Tisak, 1983) and Gardner’s (1983) intrapersonal and interpersonal intelligences, Salovey and Mayer’s (1990) original conceptualization of EI viewed the ability to process affective information as an intellectual aptitude. In keeping with Sternberg’s (1997) definition that ‘intelligence comprises the mental abilities necessary for adaptation to, as well as shaping and selection of, any environmental context’, Mayer and Salovey (1997) developed an ability model that suggests that EI abilities develop with age and experience similar to crystallized intellectual abilities. Their model emphasizes four cognitive components: the capacity to perceive emotion, to integrate it in thought, to understand emotion, and to manage emotion. 6
Within this model EI refers to a form of intelligence that involves the ability to monitor one’s own and others’ emotions, to discriminate among them, and to use the information to guide one’s thinking and actions (Salovey & Mayer, 1990). It has been suggested that individuals predominantly react and interrelate within an environment because of the way they feel and think, rather than on rationality alone. Notably, it has been argued that the competencies underpinning emotional intelligence, which enable people to demonstrate intelligent use of their emotions in managing themselves and working effectively with others, relate to workplace performance (Gardner & Stough, 2001). Yet, perhaps what is most attractive about this construct to the corporate world is that, unlike traditional models of intelligence and personality, it has been hypothesized that an individual can develop Emotional Intelligence through awareness and emotional competency programs (e.g., see Gardner & Stough, 2002). Mixed Model of Emotional Intelligence Bar-On’s (1997) mixed model of emotional intelligence, like those of previous theorists such as Goleman (1995), considered Emotional Intelligence to be comprised of emotional aspects along with other aspects that were more traditionally associated with personality. Combining emotion and personality into a single construct, Bar-On considered there to be five components of emotional intelligence, each containing numerous competencies. Bar-On’s mixed model proposed that emotional intelligence consisted of Intrapersonal intelligence, interpersonal intelligence, adaptability, stress management, and general 7
mood (Bar-On, 1997). Each of these five components consists of a number of competencies. Intrapersonal intelligence, or internal intelligence, included emotional self- awareness, assertiveness, self-regard, self-actualization and independence. Interpersonal intelligence, or external intelligence, consists of empathy, social responsibility, and interpersonal relationships. The third component, adaptability, consists of reality testing, flexibility and problem solving, with stress management comprising of stress tolerance and impulse control. The final component of Bar-On’s theory, general mood, comprised of traits usually considered to be part of personality such as optimism and happiness. While many of Bar-On’s (1997) components equate to those developed in the ability models his concept of EI is much broader. There are also significant differences in the measurement and assessment of EI within these models. The Bar-On measure like many self-report measures are often referred tounder the umbrella of mixed models of Ei and include a range of traits, dispositions, beliefs and behaviors. It is beyond the scope of the current chapter to review and critically comment on the different models of EI. This has recently been done in a sports psychology journal by Meyer and Fletcher (2007) and elsewhere in this book. However, it is noteworthy to point out that there are different models, conceptualizations and measures of EI, although there is to date no consensus as to which of these models and measures of EI are most appropriate and useful in predicting real life criteria. There are current scientific debates regarding the construct validity which may or may not be resolved in the near future. There also appears to be a clear distinction between scientific discussion on for example the construct validity of a scale of EI and the actual popularity and use of the scale in the 8
workplace. Sports psychologists will use models and measures which allow elite sports people to grasp and run with theories and ideas and which allow the quickest and most efficient hand over of information from coach or mentor to athlete. Whether a variety of different measures may work together in a synergistic manner by providing new and useful but independent understandings of the construct or simply to repeat information is not yet understood. Theoretical Linkages Between EI and Sports Psychology In attempting to link sports psychology variables to a model for both practice and research requires the adoption of a single model of EI in the first instance. Below we describe some of our thoughts in this regard with the Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence test (SUEIT). The SUEIT also referred to as GENOS EI in a previous chapter in this book (see the chapter by Palmer et al). Due to the considerable amount of overlap in theoretical content and structure of the existing theories, Palmer and Stough (2001) conducted a large factor analytic study using a population representative sample. The study involved six of the current predominant measures of emotional intelligence including the MSCEIT, the Bar-On EQI, the Trait Meta-Mood scale, the twenty-item Toronto alexythmia scale-II, the scale by Schutte et al (1998) and finally the scale by Tett et al. (1997). These 6 scales are highly representative of all the different models in Emotional intelligence (Palmer & Stough, 2001). 9
The test provides scores on five factors; 1. emotional recognition and expression: 2. emotional reasoning 3. understanding emotions of others 4. emotional management (self and others) 5. emotional control: Having acknowledged the excess of anecdotal reports regarding the similarities between successful leaders and organizations in the sport and business arenas (Weinberg & McDermott, 2002), we draw theoretical linkages between the workplace and the sporting environment when evaluating the Emotional Intelligence literature. Similarly to the workplace, sport is a highly-charged and emotional environment, and one in which inappropriate emotions may hinder performance. Yet despite bold statements such as “emotional intelligence is directly related to performance” (Abraham, 1999, p.4.) and “emotional competence is a learned capability based on emotional intelligence that results in outstanding performance at work” (Goleman, 2001, p.1.), there has been no significant theoretical links proposed between EI and sporting performance. It is often noted by coaches, sport scientists and psychologists, and the athletes themselves that the most technically gifted athletes do not always end up as the best performers (Morgan, 2003). Typically, coaches have suggested these athletes possess abilities that their less successful colleagues lack, such as supreme self-confidence, mental toughness, unshakeability and strong will (Morgan, 2003). While these qualities 10
have traditionally been categorized as attributes of a ‘true sportsperson’, perhaps a more appropriate classification, considering the growing body of literature about performance in the corporate environment, would be under the label of EI. Based upon some of the theoretical links between attentional style and sport type and studies examining Hanin’s Individual Zones of Optimal Functioning model (Hanin & Stambulova, 2002), it is possible to make some tentative hypotheses relating the dimensions of the SUEIT to the three sport categories (closed-skill, individual open- skilled and team open-skilled sport). These hypotheses are tentative, and therefore require empirical testing, as this is the first theoretical article of its type to link dimensions of the SUEIT or even any dimensions of EI to sporting classifications. Despite this, it is suggested that the EI dimension from the SUEIT, ‘understanding the emotions of others’, is best related to Nideffer and Bond’s (1990) finding that ‘awareness’ significantly contributes to the equation separating athletes on the basis of their sport type. The ‘understanding of the emotions of others’ dimension measures an individual capacity to recognize how others’ respond to their surrounding environment (Palmer & Stough, 2001). In Table 1 we propose some theoretical linkages between the 5 dimensions of the SUEIT and their relative importance for different types of sports. Table 1 About Here According to Palmer and Stough (2001), high scores on the ‘understanding the emotions of others’ dimension reflect the recognition and acknowledgement of how emotions 11
influence organizational dynamics, as well as the ability to identify the emotional ‘overtones’ of the environment. Therefore, within the sporting environment, an individual who reports high competency on this Emotional Intelligence factor may be able to read their teammate’s or opponents’ emotional response to the atmosphere of the competition. Furthermore, a high score on this dimension suggests that the athlete has a good understanding of why others in the competition are responding in a particular way and how it affects the individual or team’s performance. Nideffer and Bond (1990) found that the ‘awareness’ scores were least predictive of the closed-skilled sport category but most predictive of the individual open-skilled sport type. Thus it is hypothesized for future research, as shown in Table 1, that athletes performing open-skilled individual and team sports will be most likely to report using ‘emotional understanding’ but athletes in the closed-skill category least likely. In the example of tennis, an open-skilled individual sport, ‘reading’ the emotions of your opponent is crucial to successful performance (Anshel, 1990; Orbach, Singer, & Price, 1999). The ability to identify that your opponent is experiencing ‘negative’ emotions such as anxiety and self-doubt, allows an athlete to capitalize on their opponent’s weaknesses (Anshel, 1990). For example in tennis, the athlete may force play around the baseline having recognized that their opponent has shown frustration throughout the match at points played around this area of the court (Anshel, 1990; Orbach, et al, 1999). Conversely, by acknowledging that their opponent is experiencing ‘positive’ feelings an athlete can re-evaluate their own game-plan so as to change the dynamics of the match. Additionally, by understanding the appropriateness of an emotional response to a 12
linesman’s call for example, an athlete is able to recognize the emotional overtones of the game. By understanding the emotional dynamics of the competition, the player is able to adapt his or her play suitably. High scores on the ‘emotional reasoning’ dimension may indicate that some individuals make intuitive decisions based on feelings rather than on pure fact while others make decisions based more on analytical information (Palmer & Stough, 2001). This dimension is suggested to be similar to Nideffer and Bond’s (1990) ‘analytical’ attentional style, a characteristic found to be predictive of sport types. Therefore, athletes who score high on ‘analytical’ attentional style may score lower on the ‘emotional reasoning’ dimension of the SUEIT, indicating that they do not incorporate emotions into decisions regarding sporting performance. Speculatively we propose that scores on the ‘emotional reasoning’ dimension will be most predictive of the open-skilled team classification and least predictive of closed-skilled and open-skilled individual sports as shown in Table 1. In keeping with this hypothesis, in the open-skilled team sport of basketball it is hypothesised that it will be beneficial to performance for an athlete to incorporate emotions into decision-making when competing (Madden, Summers & Brown, 1990). Basketball requires an athlete to play intuitively and to be flexible in adapting game-plans depending on the emotions within the competitive environment (Madden, et al, 1990). That is, a successful basketballer is hypothesized to be able to quickly evaluate how different game strategies will affect play by incorporating the technical information provided to her or him during practice drills, together with the athlete’s ‘gut-feeling’ on the correct choice of play. 13
Palmer and Stough (2001) suggested that the ‘emotional management’ factor assesses the extent to which an individual is able to foster and maintain beneficial positive moods and emotions so as to effectively manage stress within oneself and others. By effectively managing one’s own emotions an individual is better able to remain task focused and avoid external and internal distractions. According to Nideffer (1990), by shifting the focus of attention from a negative internal or external source to a more positive internal focus, an athlete is less likely to perform an error. This finding is supported by Hanin’s IZOF model (2000) that states that facilitating-positive emotions help an athlete to produce energy aiding performance. Theoretically, high competency levels of ‘emotional management’ within a sporting environment will reflect an athlete’s ability to foster positive moods within themselves and their teammates, as well as effectively manage competitive anxiety levels. If making theoretical assumptions based on Nideffer and Bond’s (1990) findings, the high competency on this dimension would be most predictive of high performance in closed-skill sports and least predictive performance in open- skilled team category. Notably ‘emotional management’ has also been shown to be an important attribute of leadership within the workplace (Gardner & Stough, 2002), and therefore likely to be a dimension of EI reported by team captains. As indicated in Table 1, ‘emotional management’ is also hypothesized to be predictive of athletes who compete as individuals. Successful performance in individual closed-skill sports requires the athlete to effectively manage their own moods and anxiety levels as there is no team-mate support available. For example in the closed skill sport of diving, an athlete is assessed on the total score of a series of dives (Orlick & Partington, 1988). Hence, a diver would need 14
to effectively manage negative feelings of self-doubt and anxiety after an initial poor diving performance so as to mentally re-prepare for the following dives (Orlick & Partington, 1988). It may be argued that strong expressions of emotions such as anger, frustration, sadness and hostility are not constructive in the context of the workplace and can damage interpersonal relationships. Similarly, Hanin’s IZOF model suggests that facilitating- negative emotions cause energy production rather than utilization, and subsequently results in poorer athletic performance. Hence it is theorized that athletes who score high on the ‘emotional control’ dimension would be able to inhibit strong emotions, such as anger and hostility, from detrimentally affecting their thoughts and performance during competition. Nideffer and Bond (1990) found that the interpersonal style, ‘control’, was most predictive of individual open-skilled sport types but least predictive of open-skilled team sports. Therefore we propose that scores on the ‘emotional control’ dimension of the SUEIT will be predictive of athletes performing open-skilled individual sports but not of the open-skilled team category. In keeping with this hypothesis, it could be assumed that the open-skilled individual sport of wrestling requires the athlete to control the impact of strong emotions from detrimentally affecting their performance (Mahoney, 1989; Morgan, 1984). Undoubtedly the one-on-one competition of wrestling would elicit strong emotions such as anger and frustration, however it could be presumed that the successful 15
athlete inhibits such feelings from affecting their thoughts, actions and behaviors while competing (Mahoney, 1989). Palmer and Stough (2001) suggested that the ‘emotional recognition and expression’ dimension assesses how well you perceive your own emotions and how effectively you express your feelings to others. Therefore within the sporting environment athletes who indicate high scores on this dimension will be conscious of their emotions while competing and be able to express these emotions suitably and accurately within the performance arena. According to Hanin’s IZOF model (2000), to sustain mental and physical effort in achieving goals, facilitating-positive emotions helps the athlete to produce energy and organize functions. By accurately assessing one’s own emotions and effectively communicating those feelings, it could be assumed that an athlete is suitably organizing their emotion content to benefit performance (Hanin, 2000). Likewise by accurately displaying emotions during performance, an athlete presumably contributes to the development of a better team environment as teammates can more effectively respond to one another’s display of feelings. Druskat and Wolff (2001) suggested that team spirit is an important component of team building within the workplace. In a study of team dynamics within the workplace, Druskat and Wolff (2001) found that within the more effective teams, individuals are able to suitably express their feelings to one another and thus collaborate unreservedly. It would therefore seem that the advantages of accurately expressing one’s emotions would appear to be greater for athletes competing within a team environment as team members competent in ‘emotional recognition and expression’ would be better able to articulate issues important towards building the team’s 16
capabilities. As illustrated in Table 1 it is hypothesized that scores on this dimension would be more predictive of the open-skilled team classification and least predictive of athletes performing closed-skill sports. In the example of volleyball, it is assumed that perceiving your teammate’s feelings, as well as communicating your own emotions, would be crucial to successful performance (Leslie-Toogood & Martin, 2003; Mahoney, Gabriel & Perkins, 1987). As volleyball requires the reading of hand-signals to determine strategies of play a successful volleyballer may be particularly conscious of all their movements and expressions to their teammate (Mahoney, et al, 1987), including being overtly aware of their emotional expression. After a successful point in volleyball competition, teammates often express positive displays of emotion to one another by patting each other on the back etc (Leslie- Toogood & Martin, 2003). In doing so, they consciously indicate feelings of elation and encouragement to one another. Equally, it could be hypothesized that successful volleyballers would be very aware of how they were communicating their feelings, so as not to allow their opponents to effectively respond to their weaknesses. Potentially, Emotional Intelligence could provide additional information about sporting performance to other psychological models offering a comprehensive description about the role of emotions in competitive performance and training. Yet, perhaps what makes EI a useful addition to other psychological constructs to date is that it proposes ways to improve an athlete’s capacity to deal effectively with their own and other’s emotions. Unlike traditional intelligence theories and personality models, EI has been hypothesized 17
as a key construct that can be developed through specific emotion focused training (Greenberg, 2002). Therapeutic and preventive training programs are already in place that could be helpful in preparing elite athletes for emotional problems that could intrude on, facilitate competitive performance, prevent them, or correct them when they occur (Lazarus, 2000). Therefore it is conceivable that in the near future, sporting bodies will integrate EI into traditional sports psychology and mental training programs so as to gain that competitive edge over competitors. Another potential role of EI within the sporting arena is in the development and training of athletes for post-sport careers. The importance of EI within a successful corporate environment is increasingly being supported be organizational psychology research (Gardner & Stough, 2002). As the authors foresee that the basic skills of EI will be similar regardless of the environment, transferring those skills from one arena to another will have obvious benefit to the athlete. Recent research in the area of Athlete Career Transition supports the idea that skills learnt within the sporting environment are valuable for an athlete’s successful transition into the workforce (Lavallee & Wylleman, 2000). Obviously there is the need for a large body of empirical study to be conducted so as to establish whether there is evidence for the utility of EI in predicting performance in sport. Yet on the basis of the research examining EI and workplace performance, investigation into the relationship seems warranted. Many researchers investigating EI within the workplace have acknowledged the potential avenue of study that the sporting arena provides. However to date there has been no published work examining the relationship 18
between EI and sport. Therefore, although the above hypotheses provide potential direction for future investigation, the possible research avenues examining the role of EI in sport are seemingly numerous. Several potential areas of particular focus could be in the areas of talent identification, emotional profiles for specific sports and profiling specific positions of play within team sports. Empirical relationships between EI and Sports Psychology The above section has described some potential theoretical linkages between EI and sports psychology variables. Now we turn our attention to some linkages (albeit preliminary) between EI and sports variables in the game of professional basketball. We propose, based on linkages between EI and workplace performance as well as some of our pilot work a number of hypotheses. It was predicted that those individuals who have higher levels Emotional Management and Emotional Control, would perform better under the stresses of competitive situations. It was hypothesized that those individuals who have higher accuracy in shooting from the field, three point line and free throw line would show higher levels of Emotional Control and Emotional Management. It was also predicted that those individuals who made more defensive plays throughout the season would show higher levels of Understanding Emotions, shown in their ability to read the opposition through body language, enabling a greater number of steals, turnovers and blocked shots. Considering the importance of rebounding in team and individual performance, and due to the fact that rebounding involves an understanding of opposition players, it was predicted that those individuals who had did the majority of the rebounding, both 19
defensively and offensively, would show greater ability in understand emotions in others and the environment. Participants The sample consisted of 49 elite basketball players which comprised 31 male basketball players with ages ranging from 11 and 35 years (M= 18.26, SD=6.32), and 18 female basketball players with ages between 15 and 27 (M=18.06, SD= 2.26). Players were recruited from the South Australian Sports Institute, and several teams from the Victorian Basketball League including the Frankston Blues, Kilsyth Cobras, Bulleen Academy, and Sanderingham Sabres. Each participant was required to be either part of an elite development squad or to be playing at representative level in the VLB/SEABL 9both of which are semi-professional leagues) Materials Emotional Intelligence was measured using the Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test (SUEIT) (ά = 0.83), developed by Palmer and Stough in 1999. The SUEIT is a self-report inventory, which indexes the way people typically think, feel, and act with emotions according to the five factor model of the SUEIT. 20
Results and Discussion Descriptive statistics including means and standard deviations for the emotional intelligence measures and the five subscales, age and shooting performance accuracies are presented in Table 2 below. Table 2 About Here All of the collected variables were then correlated in order to determine whether any relationships exist between any of the basketball performance measures and emotional intelligence. Because the focus of the present research was player performance and not actual court time, correlations were calculated between performance variables and EI whilst controlling for the number of games played and the average minutes on court, as players who play more games and spend more time on court are likely to show better performance outcomes in terms of outcome variables such as total points scored etc. Table three shows the calculated correlations for all of the variables. Table 3 About Here Emotional Intelligence and Shooting Performance (Offense) To test for the prediction that players with higher levels of emotional intelligence would show greater accuracies when shooting from the field, from the three point line, and from the foul shot line, each of the three shooting accuracies were correlated with overall EI. The results indicated that there was no significant relationship between the three measures of shooting accuracy and overall emotional intelligence. Additionally shooting 21
accuracies were also correlated with the individual dimensions of emotional intelligence, and while it was predicted that emotional management and emotional control would both show positive relationships with all three shooting accuracies, only weak correlations were observed. These results may be due to the fact that the results were based on a limited number of games, with some players playing as little as 5 games. The results may also be dependent on the number of participants, and as the sample size was relatively small, there was insufficient statistical power to show significant relationships. While the shooting accuracies were the main indicators of player shooting performance, the number of shots taken along with the number of shots made were also correlated with EI, as there are times when performance is indicated not only by the accuracy of the shots taken, but also by the number of shots taken. When correlated with the number of field shots taken Emotional management showed a moderate correlation (r= .44, p= .01), indicating that players who take more shots from the field tend to have greater emotional management. Similarly when correlated with field shots taken emotional control showed a moderate- strong correlation (r= .59, p=.000), indicating that players who take more shots from their field also tend to show greater emotional control. It was also found that Emotional Management(r= .41, p= .015) and emotional control (r= .62, p=.00) both showed moderate correlations with the number of field shots made, indicating that those who are better able to control and manage their emotions make more shots from the field throughout the season. 22
When assessing the relationship between emotional intelligence, the components of emotional intelligence and shots from the three-point line it was evident that the relationships were similar to those found for field shots. The final measure of shooting performance was that of the free throw shot accuracy. Again, contrary to predictions, accuracy from the free throw line showed no significant relationships with any of the components of emotional intelligence. Emotional control (r= .43, p= .011) and emotional management (r= .43, p=.012) on the other hand did show significant correlations with the number of shots taken from the free throw line indicating that players who have the ability to control and manage their emotions generally take more shots from the three point line. Similarly, as would be expected, those who have higher levels of emotional control (r= .47, p=.005) and emotional management (r= .49, p=.004) are significantly more likely to score more points from the free throw line. It was also found that there was a strong correlation between emotional control and the total points scored and a moderate-strong correlation between emotional management and total points scored, indicating that players with higher levels of emotional control (r= .63, p= .000) and emotional management (r= .44, p= .009) are significantly more likely to score throughout the season. Emotional Intelligence, Rebounding and Defensive Plays (Defense) 23
While the main performance measure in basketball is the ability to score points, the ability to limit the number of points scored by the opposition is important. It was predicted that those who are better at understanding the emotions of others and the environment would have better defensive performance as they have the ability to read others facial expressions, body movements allowing them to predict the next move. Contrary to this expectation, there were no correlations between Understanding Emotions and the defensive plays such as defensive rebounding, blocked shots and steals. Interestingly, individuals who made more blocked shots (r=-.417, p=.014) and more steals (r=-.356, p=.031) were less able to recognize and express emotions within themselves. While it was predicted that those who are better able to understand emotions in others and the environment would show higher rebounding rates, both defensive (r= .311, p= .053) and offensive (r=.427, p=.012) rebounding correlated positively with Emotional Management, suggesting that players who are better at managing their emotions make more offensive and defensive rebounds throughout the season. As this was the first study linking basketball statistics and performance with EI, there were a number of tentative hypotheses. Despite this there were a number of statistically significant relationships between different EI dimensions and basketball statistics that suggest that EI assessment and training may be useful in elite basketball performance. 24
Table 1 Summary of Hypothesized relationship between dimensions of the SUEIT and the three sporting categories. Emotional Emotional Understanding Emotional Emotional Sport Recognition and reasoning the emotions of Management Control Expression others Type Open-skilled - - + + + Individual Open-skilled + + + - - Team Closed-skilled - - - + - 25
TABLE 2 Means and Standard Deviations of Emotional Intelligence, Age and Performance Indicators Measure Mean Standard Deviation Age 18.18 5.175 Emotional Intelligence 209.94 20.98 Emotional Recognition and Expression 36.63 4.73 Understanding Emotions External 70.12 8.42 Emotional Reasoning 36.04 4.97 Emotional Management 39.18 5.24 Emotional Control 27.95 5.05 Field Shot Accuracy 39.42 8.82 Three point Accuracy 18.01 16.41 Free Throw Accuracy 62.02 13.87 Total Points Scored 159.80 147.44 26
Table 3.: Correlations for each of the measured variables EREXP UEX EDC EM EC EI AGE .0032 .0907 .1081 .0948 .3043 .0053 SEX -.0441 -.3142 -.1362 -.1829 .1843 -.1570 FLDT .1715 .2715 -.1010 .4358* .5886** .3758* FLDM .0337 .1440 -.1451 .4088* .6240** .2913 FLDA -.2579 -.2545 -.0782 .0810 .3076 -.0680 TPST .2068 .0047 -.0182 -.1169 -.0273 .0045 TPSM .1701 .0179 -.0481 -.0661 .0479 .0270 TPSA .1933 -.0472 -.0308 -.1350 .0906 .0049 FTST .2211 .2452 -.1410 .4271* .4313* .3249* FTSM .2479 .2917 -.0606 .4868* .4736* .3921* FTSA .1717 .2576 .3045 .1380 .0177 .2410 OREB -.0058 .1879 .1015 .4274* .2856 .2735 DREB -.1577 -.0523 -.2020 .3114 .2712 .0498 TREB -.0998 .0519 -.0791 .3833* .2950 .1532 PTST .1280 .1945 -.1467 .4420* .6310** PPG -.0087 .0760 -.1936 .2875 .4998* .1834 ASS -.0090 -.0060 -.0831 -.1418 -.0938 -.1031 BLK -.4175* -.1729 -.1850 .1151 .1516 .1264 STL -.3565* -.1410 -.1549 -.1661 -.2640 -.2724 TNR .1418 .1165 -.0041 .2137 -.0530 .1131 Note * p
References Abraham, R. (1999). Emotional Intelligence in Organizations: A Conceptualization. Genetic, Social & General Psychology Monographs, 125, 209-225. Anshel, M. (1990).Toward validation of a model for coping with acute stress in sport. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 21(1), 58-83. Bond (1991) Crust, L. (2002). Emotional Control: These pre-performance strategies will take control of your emotions before they take control of you. Peak performance, 172, 1-4. Ford, S. & Summers, J. (1992). The factorial validity of the TAIS attentional-style subscales. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 14(3), 283-297. Ford, M. and Tisak, M.S. (1983). A further search for social intelligence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 196-206. Gardner, L. & Stough, C. (2001), Examining the relationship between leadership and emotional intelligence in senior level managers. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 23 (2), 68-78. Greenberg, L.S. (2002). Emotion Focused Therapy. Washington DC: American Psychological AssociationLavallee, D. & Wylleman, P. (Eds). (2000). Career Transitions in Sport: International Perspectives. USA: Fitness Information Technology Inc. 28
Leslie-Toogood, A, Martin, G. (2003), Do Coaches Know the Mental Skills of Their Athletes? Assessments from Volleyball and Track, Journal of Sport Behavior, 26 (1), 56- 69. Mayer, J.D. & Salovey, P. (1997), “What is emotional intelligence?”, in Salovey, P. and Sluyter, D.J. (Eds), Emotional Development and Emotional Intelligence, Basic Books, New York, NY. Mahoney, M. (1989). Psychological Performance of Elite and Non-Elite Performance in Olympic Wrestling. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 20, 1-12. Mahoney, M., Gabriel, T. & Perkins, T. (1987). Psychological Skills and Exceptional Athletic Performance. The Sport Psychologist, 1, 181-199. Meyer, B., & Fletcher, T. B. (2007). Emotional Intelligence: A Theoretical overview and implications for research and professional practice in sport psychology. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 1-15. Morgan, P. (???). Emotional intelligence and performance. The Coach, ??, 27-30. Morgan, W. (1984). Limits of Human Performance. In American Academy of Physical Education Papers, No. 18, Fifty-Sixth Annual Meeting, Eugene, Oregon, July, 19-26. Human Kinetics: USA. Morgan, W. (1974). Selected psychological considerations in sport. Research Quarterly, 45, 324-339. Nideffer, R.M. (1976). Test of Attentional and Interpersonal Style. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 394-404. Orbach, I, Singer, R & Price, S. (1999). An Attribution Training Program and Achievement in Sport. Sport Psychologist, 13, 69-83. 29
Palmer, B. and Stough, C. (2001). Workplace SUIET: Swinburne University Emotional Intelligence Test- Descriptive Report, Organisational Psychology Research Unit, Swinburne University; AU Orlick, T. & Partington, J. (1988). Mental Links to Excellence. The Sport Psychologist, 2, 105-130. Pfeiffer, S. (2001). Emotional Intelligence: Popular but Elusive Construct. Roeper Review, 23 (3), 138-142. Schmidt, R. (1991). Motor Learning and Performance. From Principles to Practice. Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc:USA. Schurr, K., Ashley, M. & Joy, K. (1977). AS multivariate analysis of male athlete characteristics: Sport type and success. Multivariate Experimental Clinical Research, 3, 53-68. 30
You can also read