DO WOODPECKER DRUMS ENCODE INFORMATION FOR SPECIES RECOGNITION?1
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
The Condor 103:143–150 q The Cooper Ornithological Society 2001 DO WOODPECKER DRUMS ENCODE INFORMATION FOR SPECIES RECOGNITION?1 DANIELLE J. DODENHOFF2,3, ROBERT D. STARK2 AND ERIC V. JOHNSON Biological Sciences Department, California Polytechnic State University, San Luis Obispo, CA 93407 Abstract. We investigated whether woodpecker drums are exchanged interspecifically and attempted to ascertain the drum variables responsible for species recognition. Playback experiments were conducted on four woodpecker species (Nuttall’s Picoides nuttallii, White- headed P. albolarvatus, Hairy P. villosus, and Downy P. pubescens Woodpeckers) com- paring behavioral responses to sympatric hetero- and conspecific drums. There was a sig- nificant difference in behavioral responses to sympatric hetero- versus conspecific drums; individuals responded less intensely to drums of heterospecifics when drum cadences were dissimilar. Allopatric species drums with similar cadences were used to examine whether cadence was a variable that encodes species identity. There was no significant difference between responses to allopatric hetero- versus conspecific drums with a similar cadence, indicating that the drum’s cadence encodes information for species recognition. Further playbacks eliminated drum duration and spectral properties as important variables for species recognition. Results suggest that if an individual drums at a similar cadence to a sympatric heterospecific, then their signal may elicit a conspecific response. Key words: drumming, interspecific communication, nonvocal communication, species recognition, woodpeckers. INTRODUCTION of individuals (Short 1982, Wilkins and Ritchi- Acoustic communication in birds normally oc- son 1999). Given that many of these functions curs by the use of songs and calls. However, correspond to bird song, some researchers have woodpeckers lack the ability to generate songs concluded that drumming should have similar similar to those used by passerines (Bracken- characteristics and have even postulated drum- bury 1982). Along with a vocal repertoire of ming to be the evolutionary counterpart to pas- calls, woodpeckers employ a long-distance non- serine song in woodpeckers (Pynnönen 1939, vocal acoustical signal aptly referred to as drum- Lawrence 1967), although others disagree ming. A woodpecker drum is a rapid, repetitive (Winkler and Short 1978). series of strikes with the bill on a substrate, not Although there are several accounts of inter- associated with foraging or cavity excavation specific reaction to drumming, references have (Bent 1939, Pynnönen 1939, Short 1974). tended to consolidate the variety of responses as Drumming is unusual in that a separate instru- equivalent to responses given to conspecific ment in addition to the bird’s bill is required to drums (Lawrence 1967, Winkler and Short produce the signal (Skutch 1985). 1978). The problem with comparing woodpeck- Limited information is available concerning er responses to drums stems from the lack of nonvocal acoustical signals in avian species information on the function of the multiple sig- (Prum 1998). As an instrumental signal, drum- nals woodpeckers use to communicate. There ming has been noted to be a form of long-dis- are three classes of calls (call notes, intimate tance communication that may or may not elicit calls, and rattle calls) which may be given in hetero- or conspecific responses (Crockett 1975, response to a drum, and it is unknown if these Duncan 1990). Functions attributed to drum- calls are equivalent responses to drumming ming include territorial establishment, mate at- alone. traction, pair bond maintenance, and localization There is no consensus as to whether drums encode species-specific information detectable 1 Received 2 March 2000. Accepted 27 September by receivers. Four hypotheses have been sug- 2000. gested. First, drums are species-specific and not 2 Present address: Department of Evolution, Ecology and Organismal Biology, The Ohio State University, normally exchanged interspecifically (Perrins Columbus, OH 43210. and Middleton 1985, Welty and Baptista 1988). 3 E-mail: dodenhoff.1@postbox.acs.ohio-state.edu Second, drums are not distinctive because other [143]
144 DANIELLE J. DODENHOFF ET AL. TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics of the drum variables (mean 6 SD) for the playback tapes. Cadence Duration Interstrike Playback signal (strikes sec21) (sec.) No. strikes interval (sec.) Nuttall’s 20.8 6 0.4 1.03 6 0.1 21.5 6 0.7 0.050 6 0.001 ‘‘Slow’’ Nuttall’sa 17.1 6 0.5 0.78 6 0.2 13.1 6 3.2 0.064 6 0.001 Downy 15.8 6 0.4 0.79 6 0.4 12.5 6 0.7 0.066 6 0.002 Hairy 26.6 6 0.5 0.83 6 0.3 22.0 6 7.1 0.039 6 0.001 Northern Flicker 19.3 6 1.2 0.99 6 0.4 19.3 6 7.8 0.055 6 0.005 White-headed 20.1 6 0.5 1.64 6 0.2 33.0 6 2.0 0.048 6 0.001 Three-toed 15.3 6 0.6 0.78 6 0.1 12.0 6 1.0 0.070 6 0.007 Black-backed 15.6 6 0.3 1.83 6 0.1 28.5 6 0.7 0.066 6 0.002 Ladder-backed 31.1 6 1.2 1.13 6 0.1 35.0 6 1.2 0.033 6 0.001 Pulse tone 400 Hz 15.9 1.3 20 0.066 Nuttall’s altered to Hairy 26.3 1.0 26 0.041 Hairy altered to Nuttall’s 18.8 1.0 19 0.053 a Drums . 2 SD below species average. signals, perhaps rattle calls, are used for species other species were used as playback stimuli: identification (Short 1974, 1982, Winkler and Black-backed (P. arcticus), Three-toed (P. tri- Short 1978). Third, drums are fairly species-spe- dactylus), and Ladder-backed Woodpeckers (P. cific even to the human ear, but woodpeckers scalaris), and the Northern Flicker (Colaptes au- may react indiscriminately (Winkler et al. 1995). ratus). Determinations of sympatry were based Fourth, drums are sympatrically, but not allo- on each species’ breeding range within sampled patrically, species-specific, with the drum ca- areas. White-headed Woodpeckers could be con- dence (in strikes sec21) as the predominant var- sidered marginally sympatric with Nuttall’s and iable for species identification (Stark et al. Downy Woodpeckers, but only during nonbreed- 1998). ing months (Beedy and Granholm 1985, Gaines Two unresolved questions remain in the afore- 1988). mentioned hypotheses. First, whether responses The following procedure was used for all to hetero- versus conspecific drums are different, playback trials: Subject birds were observed for and whether the variable(s) used by woodpeck- a minimum of 5 min before each trial. Only in- ers for species recognition are contained within dividuals that were foraging, preening, or cavity- the drum. We designed three experiments to re- excavating, and within acoustical range were solve these questions. Experiment 1 established used. Playback experiments were conducted in a method of scoring responses to drums, and ex- a random, balanced, pairwise design (conspecif- periment 2 compared behavioral responses to ic:heterospecific), with a minimum of 30 min playbacks of sympatric hetero- versus conspe- between stimuli (based on log survivorship cific drums. Experiment 3 compared behavioral curves calculated for the maximum natural du- responses to playbacks of allopatric heterospe- ration a bird remained responsive during drum- cific and computer-generated drums versus con- ming sequences, Dodenhoff 1996). Birds unre- specific drums, to test possible variables used for sponsive to both stimuli were excluded from the species recognition. analysis. Drum variables for playback tapes were based METHODS on those reported by Stark et al. (1998), and in- GENERAL PLAYBACK DESIGN cluded duration (sec), number of strikes per Populations in four regions of California, in- drum, cadence, and interstrike interval in one cluding the Los Padres, Sequoia, Sierra, and drum (interstrike interval, in seconds; Table 1). Inyo National Forests, were sampled during the Tapes were generated by editing together field 1993–1996 breeding seasons. Four species were recordings and commercially available drums studied using playbacks: Nuttall’s (Picoides nut- (Cornell Laboratory of Ornithology 1992a, tallii), Hairy (P. villosus), Downy (P. pubes- 1992b), computer generated tones, and altered cens), and White-headed Woodpeckers (P. al- natural drums reset to specific cadences. Play- bolarvatus). In addition, the drums from four back tapes were standardized for decibel level.
DO WOODPECKER DRUMS ENCODE INFORMATION FOR SPECIES RECOGNITION? 145 The results from the analysis of behavioral servable changes from one behavior to another, observations were used to construct a scoring were noted for each individual during a drum- method for comparing responses to playback ming sequence (adapted from Halloran and Be- signals for experiments 2 and 3. Although there koff 1995). Behaviors that significantly preceded are differences between calls and displays of or followed drumming were determined by se- woodpeckers, stereotypical behaviors are ob- quence analysis using a first-order Markov served within the family Picidae (Winkler et al. chain. 1995). Therefore, generalizations from obser- Independence between consecutive behaviors vations of Nuttall’s Woodpecker correlated with was tested using categorical modeling proce- behaviors observed for other subject species. We dures (SAS Institute 1990), which used maxi- used these results to rank the behavioral data mum likelihood values to estimate the G-test sta- appropriately concerning responsiveness to tistic and its associated x2 statistic (Sokal and drum playbacks. Rohlf 1981). Data collection excluded the pos- Behaviors observed more often during drum- sibility of behaviors following themselves. ming sequences from experiment 1 were used to Therefore, the ‘‘expected’’ values where the pre- create a categorical scoring method similar to ceding and following behavior were the same Emlen (1972) for experiments 2 and 3. Behav- were considered ‘‘logical zeros,’’ and the ex- iors included alarm calls, approach, drumming, flights over the speaker, display, attack/supplant, pected values were calculated using a log-linear and frozen antipredation posture. These behav- model (Fagen and Young 1978). Significant dif- iors composed the intensity scale. The intensity ferences were detected when the transformed scale ranked the behavioral level achieved dur- frequency zYz . 1.08. ing each playback and ranged from 0–7 (low– To investigate which behaviors occurred more high). Given the subjective nature of categorical often during drumming sequences versus other scoring, we also employed a hybrid scale, which activities, a comparison of the frequency of each subdivided the intensity scale into differing lev- behavior was conducted using a x2 analysis for els of response (using frequency data), weighing contingency tables. Twelve different Nuttall’s each of the seven categories equally. Hybrid Woodpeckers were observed for 30–60 min scores ranged from 0–14 (low–high). Behavioral each during the months of March and April, responses were compared using two-tailed Wil- 1993, during non-drumming sequences. Behav- coxon signed-ranks tests. For the test to be able iors observed were classified categorically and to detect significance at the a 5 0.05 level, n compared to the behaviors observed during the must be at least 6. If n 5 6, then differences had drumming sequences. to be of like sign (Sokal and Rohlf 1981). EXPERIMENT 2: SYMPATRIC SPECIES EXPERIMENT 1: BEHAVIORAL SEQUENCE PLAYBACKS ANALYSIS Behavioral responses of the four subject species Since there were no previous measurements of to sympatric hetero- and conspecific drums were drum responses for these species, we conducted compared. Trials with Nuttall’s Woodpeckers in- a behavioral sequence analysis to categorize be- cluded drums from three sympatric species havioral responses to drumming. Observations of Nuttall’s Woodpeckers during natural and in- (Downy and Hairy Woodpeckers, and Northern duced drumming sequences were made in the Flickers), and trials with Downy Woodpeckers Los Padres National Forest, San Luis Obispo included drums from three sympatric species County, California during the 1993–1995 breed- (Nuttall’s and Hairy Woodpeckers, and Northern ing seasons. Behaviors were identified according Flickers). Northern Flicker drums used for play- to previous descriptions (Short 1971, Winkler back were recorded from individuals sympatric and Short 1978) and categorized for analysis. with Nuttall’s Woodpeckers. Hairy Woodpecker Behaviors for this analysis included foraging, trials were conducted using the drums from two flying or changing locations, intimate calls, sympatric species (Nuttall’s and Downy Wood- preening, rattle calls, look around (with bill peckers), whereas White-headed Woodpecker wave), call notes, agonistic attacks, and copu- trials consisted of drums from a single sympatric lations. Behavioral transitions, defined as ob- species (Hairy Woodpecker).
146 DANIELLE J. DODENHOFF ET AL. EXPERIMENT 3: ALLOPATRIC AND quences to its occurrence during non-drumming COMPUTER GENERATED PLAYBACKS sequences. The behaviors ‘‘preening’’ and ‘‘rat- Behavioral responses of three subject species to tle call’’ were observed more often during drum- allopatric hetero- and conspecific drums were ming sequences (x29 5 183, P , 0.01). Rattle compared. Trials with Nuttall’s Woodpecker in- calls have been noted to elicit drums, and are cluded four allopatric species drums, one with a considered similar in function to drumming similar cadence (White-headed Woodpecker) (Short 1971, Winkler and Short 1978). Although and three with different cadences (Ladder- ‘‘look around with bill wave’’ did not always backed, Black-backed, and Three-toed Wood- precede or follow drumming, it was observed peckers). Trials with White-headed Woodpeck- more often during drumming sequences. Forag- ers included playbacks with Nuttall’s Wood- ing was observed during both sequences more peckers, whereas trials with Downy Woodpeck- than expected. Previously, call notes have been ers included three allopatric species: two with considered a conspecific response to drumming similar cadences (Black-backed and Three-toed (Short 1982, Kilham 1983). However, our anal- Woodpecker) and one with a dissimilar cadence ysis indicated call notes were observed frequent- (White-headed Woodpecker). Although Black- ly in non-drumming rather than drumming se- backed and Three-toed Woodpeckers’ drums are quences. Furthermore, these call notes preceded similar in cadence to Downy Woodpeckers foraging and were more often observed during (Short 1982), they differ in the spacing pattern non-drumming activities. Thus, call notes with- of individual strikes; Black-backed and Three- out supporting behaviors were not considered a toed Woodpeckers increase cadence towards the response to drumming. end of their drum, whereas Downy Woodpeckers decrease (Stark et al. 1998). Also, the drum of EXPERIMENTS 2 AND 3: RESPONSE TO Black-backed Woodpeckers is significantly lon- PLAYBACKS ger than Downy Woodpecker drums (Stark et al. 1998). Individuals demonstrated a significant difference Because behavioral responsiveness to allopat- in the level of response between sympatric het- ric drums may be observed, computer-generated ero- versus conspecific drums, except between drums were used to test whether drum cadence Nuttall’s Woodpecker and the Northern Flicker was the primary variable responsible for species (Table 2). Furthermore, both Downy and Nut- recognition (as predicted by Stark et al. 1998). tall’s Woodpeckers were similar in their respons- Two types of trials were conducted using com- es to the Nuttall’s playback tape generated from puter-generated drums to test spectral properties. an individual greater than 2 standard deviations First, a 400-Hz tone was repeated to simulate a below the mean cadence for Nuttall’s Wood- drumming Downy Woodpecker. Second, a Nut- peckers (‘‘slow’’ Nuttall’s, Stark 1996). tall’s Woodpecker strike was set at a cadence Responses to conspecific drums were not sig- similar to a Hairy Woodpecker drum, and recip- nificantly different from responses to allopatric rocally, a Hairy Woodpecker strike was set at a species, provided the drum cadences of the two cadence similar to a Nuttall’s Woodpecker drum. species were comparable. Conversely, responses to allopatric species with dissimilar cadences RESULTS were of significantly lower intensity. The results EXPERIMENT 1: BEHAVIORAL SEQUENCE for the hybrid scale were similar to those for the ANALYSIS intensity scale (Table 2), except for a marginal Nuttall’s Woodpeckers (n 5 68 birds, with 574 difference in the Hairy versus Ladder-backed transitions) were observed for the behavioral se- Woodpecker comparison; there was a significant quence analysis. The sample size was large difference for the intensity scale but a nonsig- enough for the x2 distribution to approximate the nificant difference for the hybrid scale (Table 2). G distribution. Results of the categorical model Hairy Woodpeckers responded to Ladder-backed implied a strong dependency between behaviors Woodpecker drums, but less intensely than to a (x229 5 190, P , 0.01). Twelve Nuttall’s Wood- conspecific signal (although the hybrid scale peckers were used to analyze behavioral re- score approached significance). The results from sponses to drums by comparing each behavior’s playbacks with computer-generated signals in- frequency of occurrence during drumming se- dicated no significant difference between the re-
DO WOODPECKER DRUMS ENCODE INFORMATION FOR SPECIES RECOGNITION? 147 TABLE 2. Wilcoxon signed-ranks test results for comparisons of responses to sympatric, allopatric, and com- puter-generated drum signals versus the conspecific signal. Nonsignificant results indicate no significant differ- ences in responses to conspecific and heterospecific signals. Listed are P-values (n birds). Int 5 Intensity scale; Hyb 5 Hybrid scale; see Methods for a description of the two scales. Species Playback signal Scale Nuttall’s Downy Hairy White-headed Nuttall’s Int 0.01 (8) 0.04 (9) 0.58 (10) Hyb 0.01 (8) 0.02 (9) 0.61 (10) ‘‘Slow’’ Nuttall’sa Int 0.23 (9) 1.00 (9) Hyb 0.17 (9) 0.83 (9) Downy Int 0.01 (29) 0.01 (11) Hyb 0.01 (29) 0.01 (11) Hairy Int 0.01 (20) 0.01 (11) 0.02 (7) Hyb 0.01 (20) 0.01 (11) 0.02 (7) Northern Flicker Int 0.16 (18) 0.04 (6) Hyb 0.29 (18) 0.04 (6) White-headed Int 0.40 (19) 0.02 (8) Hyb 0.08 (19) 0.02 (8) Three-toed Int 0.03 (11) 0.18 (13) Hyb 0.01 (11) 0.23 (13) Black-backed Int 0.03 (13) 1.00 (12) Hyb 0.01 (13) 0.55 (12) Ladder-backed Int 0.04 (6) 0.04 (13) Hyb 0.04 (6) 0.06 (13) Pulse tone 400 Hz Int 0.06 (7) Hyb 0.06 (7) Nuttall’s altered to Int 1.00 (7) Hairy Hyb 0.89 (7) Hairy altered to Nut- Int 0.28 (8) tall’s Hyb 0.25 (8) a Drums more than 2 SD below species average. sponses of woodpeckers to these signals versus These results differ from Duncan (1990), be- conspecific drums. cause Duncan’s comparisons did not include re- ciprocal playbacks with species sharing similar DISCUSSION cadences. Although most researchers agree drums have Our results between species that share similar species-typical characteristics (Short 1982, Kil- cadences indicated interspecific responses may ham 1983, Winkler et al. 1995), it was unknown occur between sympatric species. The responses whether observed interspecific responses to by Nuttall’s Woodpeckers to the drums of North- drums were predictable. Previous studies includ- ern Flickers were similar to responses elicited ed multiple responses as equivalent indications by a conspecific drum. Thus, interspecific re- of species recognition (Short 1982, Winkler et sponses may occur between these sympatric spe- al. 1995), which implied ambiguity within the cies. Individual drums of both species often have drum, but did not indicate the absence of dis- a similar drum cadence, and were predicted to crimination. Individuals may respond to a het- have reciprocal heterospecific ambiguity in their erospecific drum, but those responses can differ drum (Stark et al. 1998). Despite this prediction, from responses to conspecific drums, which we did not observe interspecific responses to could indicate discrimination between stimuli. natural (i.e., non-playback) drums between in- Results from the playbacks of the ‘‘slow’’ Nut- dividuals of these species over the course of this tall’s stimulus to Nuttall’s and Downy Wood- study. We attributed the lack of interspecific re- peckers indicated that interspecific behavioral sponse to observations of restricted drumming responses to drums can occur naturally between by Northern Flickers. Northern Flickers within sympatric species, if the drum cadences overlap. the study areas drummed for a few weeks during
148 DANIELLE J. DODENHOFF ET AL. the breeding season, usually when Nuttall’s species identity. However, these results cannot Woodpeckers were unresponsive to drums. This exclude the possibility that either interstrike in- may indicate a shift between species in the tim- terval or duration encodes species identity for ing of drumming during the breeding season; Downy Woodpeckers that are sympatric with species with similar drums could reduce ambi- Black-backed or Three-toed Woodpeckers. guity through temporal separation and maintain Computer-generated drums tested whether species distinctiveness (Stacier et al. 1996). spectral properties were important in species Allopatric playbacks followed the same pat- identification. Results from trials with Nuttall’s, tern as observed in sympatric playbacks: drums Hairy, and Downy Woodpeckers indicated that of species with similar cadences elicited behav- spectral properties of a drum were not charac- ioral responses equivalent to conspecific signals, teristics responsible for species recognition. In- whereas those with dissimilar cadences did not. stead, the spectral properties generated in drum- This indicates that drum cadence is one variable ming have been noted to depend on the drum- that encodes species identity. Playback trials ming substrate selected (Eberhardt 1997). Fur- with Downy Woodpeckers tested whether the thermore, differential signal attenuation of variables ‘‘interstrike interval’’ and ‘‘duration’’ frequencies through an environment may make were used to encode species identity. Downy the spectral properties of a drum a poor variable Woodpeckers responded similarly to conspecific, for species-specificity and signaler identification Black-backed and Three-toed Woodpecker (Wiley and Richards 1982). However, these drums, which indicated that drum duration and spectral properties may be important in other as- interstrike interval were not used by Downy pects of communication, including ranging Woodpeckers to differentiate between these spe- (Morton 1986, Naugib 1995, 1998). cies. This is similar to the finding that, for many In summary, our results support the conclu- passerines, signal duration is usually not a spe- sion that the drum’s cadence encodes species cies-typical variable (Becker 1982). However, identity of the signaler, and that receivers per- because heterospecific drums used as playbacks ceive these differences within the acoustical en- were no shorter than Downy Woodpecker vironment. Individual woodpeckers did not react drums, our test does not eliminate the possibility indiscriminately to drums. Responses to heter- of a minimum duration required for species rec- ospecific drums with dissimilar cadences were ognition. Shorter song durations may elicit less less intense and did not include vocalizations or intense responses, and minimum durations are behaviors observed during playbacks with con- required for species recognition for passerines specific drums. Sympatric woodpecker drums (Becker 1982). Our results also eliminated the interstrike in- are not usually exchanged interspecifically, but terval differences in drumming as a character- interspecific responses may result if individuals istic responsible for species recognition in from different species have an overlapping drum Downy Woodpeckers. Kaiser (1990) suggested cadence. However, due to the amount of overlap that the spacing pattern within unmodulated between species, both sympatric and allopatric, drums was related to the woodpecker’s foraging we conclude drums are not species specific. The method. In ‘‘wood-pecking’’ species the rhythm lack of signal specificity does not prevent the accelerates, whereas ‘‘gleaners’’ slow down to- encoding of species identity. For example, dif- ward the end or the rhythm and amplitude of ferential timing of drumming between species their drums remain uniform (Winkler et al. within a breeding season could result in a lack 1995). Although Kaiser’s hypothesis was based of selection pressure on drums to significantly on small sample sizes, it suggested that the spac- diverge. This may allow a few sympatric species ing pattern within a drum may be controlled by to utilize similar cadences to encode species the musculature used for foraging. Thus, this identity without resulting in ambiguity between variable would have a greater likelihood of over- species. Nonetheless, given the similarities be- lapping with a sympatric species with a similar tween allopatric species, it is unknown whether foraging strategy. Given the high degree of over- the trend of signal divergence observed in this lap between the foraging strategies of many study is applicable across broad geographic sympatric woodpeckers, the interstrike interval ranges or under differing acoustical environ- of a drum may not be a reliable indicator of ments. Clearly, considerable research still needs
DO WOODPECKER DRUMS ENCODE INFORMATION FOR SPECIES RECOGNITION? 149 to be done in the area of avian mechanical quences, p. 79–113. In P. Colgan [ED.], Quantita- acoustical signals, including woodpecker drums. tive ethology. John Wiley and Sons, New York. GAINES, D. 1988. Birds of Yosemite and the east slope. Artemisia Press, Lee Vining, CA. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS HALLORAN, M., AND M. BEKOFF. 1995. Cheek rubbing We thank D. Frey, R. Gambs, W. Koenig, D. Nelson, as grooming by Abert squirrels. Anim. Behav. 50: and two anonymous reviewers for helpful comments 987–993. which significantly improved this manuscript; J. Daly, KAISER, M. 1990. Untersuchungen zur Biomechanik R. Schmidt, and R. Peck for assistance with the statis- des Balztrommelns der Specte (Picidae). Beitr. tics; and the Biological Sciences Department and De- Vogelkd. 36:129–159. partment of Statistics, California Polytechnic State KILHAM, L. 1983. Woodpeckers of eastern North University, San Luis Obispo for use of equipment, fa- America. Dover, New York. cilities, and for financial support of this research. We LAWRENCE, L. DE K. 1967. A comparative life-history especially thank R. Bowker for technical assistance study of four species of woodpeckers. Ornithol. and sound-analysis software. Monogr. 5. MORTON, E. S. 1986. Predictions from the ranging hy- pothesis for the evolution of long-range commu- LITERATURE CITED nication. Behavior 99:65–86. BECKER, P. 1982. The coding of species-specific char- NAUGIB, M. 1995. Auditory distance assessment of acteristics in bird sounds, p. 214–244. In D. singing conspecifics in Carolina wrens: the role of Kroodsma and E. Miller [EDS.], Acoustic com- reverberation and frequency-dependent attenua- munication in birds. Vol. 1. Academic Press, New tion. Anim. Behav. 50:1297–1307. York. NAUGIB, M. 1998. Perception and degradation in BEEDY, E., AND S. GRANHOLM. 1985. Discovering Si- acoustical signals and its implications for ranging. erra birds: western slope. Yosemite Nat. Hist. As- Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 42:139–142. soc. and Seqouia Nat. Hist. Assoc. Mariposa, CA. PERRINS, C. M., AND A. L. A. MIDDLETON. 1985. The BENT, A. C. 1939. Life histories of North American encyclopedia of birds. Oxford Univ. Press, New woodpeckers. U.S. Natl. Mus. Bull. 174. York. BISHOP, Y. M. M., S. E. FIENBURG, AND P. W. HOLLAND. PRUM, R. O. 1998. Sexual selection and the evolution 1975. Discrete multivariate analysis: theory and of mechanical sound production in manakins practice. M.I.T. Press, Cambridge, MA. (Aves: Pipridae). Anim. Behav. 55:977–994. BRACKENBURY, J. 1982. The structural basis of voice PYNNÖNEN, A. 1939. Beitrage zur Kenntnis der Biol- production and its relationship to sound charac- ogie finnischer Spechte. Ann. Zool. Soc. Zool. teristics, p. 53–73. In D. Kroodsma and E. Miller Bot. Fenn. Vanamo 7:1–166. [EDS.], Acoustic communication in birds. Vol. 1. SAS INSTITUTE INC. 1990. SAS user’s manual. Version Academic Press, New York. 6, 4th ed. SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC. CORNELL LABORATORY OF ORNITHOLOGY. 1992a. Inter- SHORT, L. 1971. Systematics and behavior of some active audio. A field guide to bird songs, Eastern North American woodpeckers, genus Picoides and Central edition. 2nd ed. Houghton Mifflin, (Aves). Bull. Am. Mus. Nat. Hist. 145:1–118. Boston. SHORT, L. 1974. Habits and interactions of North CORNELL LABORATORY OF ORNITHOLOGY. 1992b. Inter- American Three-toed Woodpeckers (Picoides arc- active audio. A field guide to bird songs, Western ticus and Picoides tridactylus). Am. Mus. Novi- edition. 2nd ed. Houghton Mifflin, Boston. tates 2547:1–42. CROCKETT, A. B. 1975. Ecology and behavior of the SHORT, L. 1982. Woodpeckers of the world. Delaware Williamson’s Sapsucker in Colorado. Ph.D. diss., Mus. Nat. Hist., Greenville, DE. Univ. Colorado, Boulder, CO. DODENHOFF, D. J. 1996. Interspecific and intraspecific SKUTCH, A. F. 1985. Life of the woodpecker. Ibis Publ., communication: a quantitative analysis of drum- Santa Monica, CA. ming behavior using four species of California- SOKAL, R. R., AND F. J. ROHLF. 1981. Biometry. W. H. occurring woodpeckers (Family Picidae). M.Sc. Freeman, San Francisco. thesis, California Polytechnic St. Univ., San Luis STACIER, C. A., D. A. SPECTOR, AND A. G. HORN. 1996. Obispo, CA. The dawn chorus and other diel patterns in acous- DUNCAN, S. 1990. Auditory communication in breed- tical signaling, p. 339–355. In D. Kroodsma and ing Northern Flickers (Colaptes auratus). Ph.D. E. Miller [EDS.], Ecology and evolution of acous- diss., Univ. Wisconsin, Milwaukee, WI. tic communication in birds. Cornell Univ. Press, EBERHARDT, L. S. 1997. A test of an environmental Ithaca, NY. advertisement hypothesis for the function of STARK, R. D. 1996. Minimizing signal equivocation: a drumming in Yellow-bellied Sapsuckers. Condor quantitative analysis of the drumming patterns of 99:798–803. eleven species of California occurring woodpeck- EMLEN, S. T. 1972. An experimental analysis of the ers: (Family Picidae). M.Sc. thesis, California parameters of bird song eliciting species recogni- Polytechnic State Univ., San Luis Obispo, CA. tion. Behaviour 41:130–171. STARK, R. D., D. J. DODENHOFF, AND E. V. JOHNSON. FAGEN, R., AND D. YOUNG. 1978. Temporal patterns of 1998. A quantitative analysis of woodpecker behavior: durations, intervals, latencies, and se- drumming. Condor 100:350–356.
150 DANIELLE J. DODENHOFF ET AL. WELTY, J. C., AND L. BAPTISTA. 1988. The life of birds. tion and possible causation. J. Field Ornithol. 70: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Orlando, FL. 578–586. WILEY, R. H., AND D. G. RICHARDS. 1982. Adaptation for WINKLER, H., AND L. SHORT. 1978. A comparative acoustic communication in birds: sound transmission analysis of acoustical signals in pied woodpeckers and signal detection, p. 132–183. In D. Kroodsma (Aves, Picoides). Bull. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist. 160: and E. H. Miller [EDS.], Acoustic communication in 1–109. birds. Vol. 1. Academic Press, New York. WINKLER, H., D. CHRISTIE, AND D. NURNEY. 1995. WILKINS, H. D., AND G. RITCHISON. 1999. Drumming Woodpeckers: an identification guide to the wood- and tapping by Red-bellied Woodpeckers: descrip- peckers of the world. Houghton Mifflin, Boston.
You can also read