Botai and the Origins of Horse Domestication
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Journal of Anthropological Archaeology 18, 29 –78 (1999) Article ID jaar.1998.0332, available online at http://www.idealibrary.com on Botai and the Origins of Horse Domestication Marsha A. Levine McDonald Institute for Archaeological Research, University of Cambridge, Downing Street, Cambridge, CB2 3ER, United Kingdom Received April 22, 1997; revision received April 27, 1998; accepted July 28, 1998 This paper explores some issues related to the origins of horse domestication. First, it focuses on methodological problems relevant to existing work. Then, ethnoarchaeological and archaeo- zoological methods are used to provide an alternative approach to the subject. Ethnological, ethological, and archaeological data are used to construct a series of population structure models illustrating a range of human– horse relationships. Analysis of assemblages from the Eneolithic sites of Botai (northern Kazakhstan) and Dereivka (Ukraine) suggests that horses at these sites were obtained largely by hunting. © 1999 Academic Press Key Words: archaeology; Eurasian steppe; horse; domestication. 1.0. INTRODUCTION ently intensified. For a long time archae- ologists assumed that intensification meant The impact on human society of the domestication. However, there are other earliest domestication of the horse must explanations for this kind of change which have been as profound as that of the in- must also be explored. It is important to vention of the steam engine and yet we be aware that human– horse relation- know very little about when, where, or ships varied widely over time and space how it came about. The increased mobility and that multiple relationships could be provided by the horse would have en- relevant at a single site. Furthermore, be- abled people to move further and faster havioral patterns for which we have no and to take more with them than ever modern or ethnographic analogues are before. They could exploit larger and likely to have been important in the past. more diverse landscapes, maintain larger Whatever else is involved, it is clear that families, increase the range of their trade there was an important change in steppe contacts. They could move into previously ecodynamics at this time (from around uninhabitable regions. And, since a man 5000 to 3000 B.C.). Horses were becoming on foot is no match for a man on horse- much more common in archaeological de- back, the military implications of horse posits. Important cultural, social, and eco- domestication would have been revolu- nomic changes were taking place. It was tionary. John Ewers has shown how pro- also a period of significant climatic change foundly the introduction of the horse into (Schnirelman 1992). Until we can under- North America changed Blackfoot culture stand the development of the human–horse (Ewers 1955). We should expect no less of relationship we cannot know how all these its early domestication in central Eurasia. factors were related (Levine 1993). If we are However, until recently relatively little at- to make sense of events during this period, tention had been paid to this problem (see we must understand the structure of the also Levine 1990, 1993). archaeozoological data. Around 7000 years ago the relation- The study of human– horse relation- ship between people and horses appar- ships has been bedeviled by both concep- 29 0278-4165/99 $30.00 Copyright © 1999 by Academic Press All rights of reproduction in any form reserved.
30 MARSHA A. LEVINE tual and methodological weaknesses. This posits should not be taken as proof that paper explores some of the relevant issues they were extinct. Grigson would have from two perspectives. First, it focuses on been on firmer ground had she entitled some of the problems relevant to existing her paper: The Earliest Horses in the Le- work, in particular, the confusion of do- vant? New Finds from the Fourth Millen- mestication with intensification and the nium of the Negev. use of a single criterion to classify complex human–animal relationships. Then, eth- 1.1.2. The Search for the Earliest Date noarchaeological and archaeozoological methods are used to provide an alterna- This kind of problem arises partly out of tive approach. A range of behaviors, the tendency of archaeologists and ar- based on archaeological, ethological, and chaeozoologists to ask certain kinds of ethnographic data, are drawn on to de- questions, for example, when and where velop a series of models describing a was the horse (or, for that matter, cow, range of possible strategies and tactics sheep, goat, pig, etc.) first domesticated? against which the archaeological data can Which came first: the invention of the be tested. wheel or the bit (Anthony and Brown 1991)? The whole issue of earliest dates is 1.1. The Concept of the Earliest Date a red herring, especially in a situation 1.1.1. Biogeographic Range such as this, in which the number of well- excavated and absolutely dated sites is C. Grigson’s paper, “The Earliest Do- very small and the criteria used to prove mestic Horses in the Levant? New finds domestication are not very convincing. from the Fourth Millennium of the Ne- Factors completely unrelated to ancient gev” (1993), illustrates what is probably human behavior that will significantly in- the most fundamental problem associated fluence identification of the “earliest” site with the study of early horse domestica- include the following: tion—the search for the earliest date. She natural taphonomic factors: the destruc- might well be correct, on the basis of its tion or preservation of sites, bones, and large size relative to the ass (Equus asinus) artifacts made from organic materials; and the onager (Equus hemionus), that decisions, which may be political, finan- Equus caballus was present in the Levant cial, or strategic, about where and how earlier than had been believed. However, carefully to excavate; her conviction that this horse must have decisions about whether bones should been domesticated is apparently based be studied or discarded; solely on the assumption that the geo- decisions about who will study the graphical range of the wild horse could bones, whether their primary training is not have extended into the Levant: “Al- as an archaeologist, zoologist, veterinar- though the horse (Equus caballus) was a ian, etc.; member of the Pleistocene fauna of the the specialist’s country of origin, since Levant, it died out before the end of the educational traditions influence the ana- period” (Grigson 1993, p. 646). In fact, re- lytical methods used; cent research suggests that the natural criteria chosen by the specialist as evi- distribution of the Holocene horse might dence of domestication. have been much wider than had been for- merly believed (Azzaroli 1985; Clason Scholars looking for earliest dates com- 1988; Clutton-Brock 1992; Groves 1986; monly use only one line of evidence (e.g., Uerpmann 1990). In any case, the absence biogeography, size, morphology, bitwear) of horse remains from archaeological de- from which to draw their conclusions.
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 31 This approach, simplifying human and dalenian horses were smaller than those animal behavior, as it does, is ultimately from the intervening Upper Perigordian unsatisfying. level. No one could seriously suggest that this is evidence that the former were do- 1.1.3. Size and Homogeneity mesticated while the latter were wild (Le- vine 1979, 1983). Setting the search for the “earliest date” A decrease in size accompanied by an as a primary research goal makes it rather increase in heterogeneity might be asso- easy to use inadequate criteria for the de- ciated with domestication, but it could termination of domestication. This is be- have other causes. It is, on its own, insuf- cause the search for a date does not re- ficient as an explanation. Other corrobo- quire anything to be said about the actual rative evidence must be obtained. Even if relationship between animals and people. too few teeth were available for a full- For instance, a decrease in size and an blown population analysis, a study that increase in heterogeneity are taken as compared aging data from a series of rel- proof of domestication by many scholars. atively small samples would surely be just Uerpmann claims that “Grössenreduktion as meaningful as one comparing morpho- einerseits und Zunahme der Variabilität metric data from a series of small samples. andererseits sind klassische Domestika- The latter, but not the former, are used by tionsindikatoren” (Uerpmann 1990, p. Uerpmann and others (e.g., Uerpmann 127). 1,* Such factors as age and sex struc- 1990; Benecke 1993). ture are rarely taken into account. How- ever, a size change could also result from 1.1.4. Bitwear a change in the technique of exploitation. For example, a hunting method that Another example of this commitment to culled primarily stallions from family an earliest date is Anthony’s argument groups would take larger horses than one that the domesticated horse was present that focused on bachelor groups, which in the Ukraine earlier than in Kazakhstan. might well be epiphyseally mature but not His evidence for this comes from bitwear yet full grown, or one that focused on studies of two samples of lower second females, which are smaller than equal-age premolars from two Eneolithic sites, Botai males. Environmental change, geographi- in northern Kazakhstan (5 from a total of cal isolation, and genetic drift are all con- 19 teeth) and Dereivka in the Ukraine (2 nected with size change. Moreover, ta- from a total of 6 teeth). He implies from phonomic factors can also influence size this that horse domestication spread from range and variability. For example, as an west to east (Anthony 1995). animal ages, even after its epiphyses are Relatively little archaeozoological re- fully fused, the bones continue to increase search has been carried out in the in density. All other things being equal, former Soviet Union, including both Ka- the denser the bone, the better its chances zakhstan and the Ukraine, and relatively of surviving in an archaeological context. few absolute dates are available (regard- Poor preservation conditions therefore ing the Ukraine, see Levine and Rassa- tend to result in an assemblage of rela- makin 1996). Botai and Dereivka do not tively homogeneous and large bones. At constitute representative samples of the French Upper Palaeolithic site of So- sites within the vast regions in question. lutré, both the Aurignacian and the Mag- They cannot, therefore, be used to an- swer questions about origins and earli- * See Notes section at end of paper for all foot- est dates. Moreover, serious doubts have notes. been raised about the stratigraphic loca-
32 MARSHA A. LEVINE tion of the “ritual” skull from Dereivka, TABLE 1 the basis of Anthony and Brown’s theory Dereivka Radiocarbon Dates of the origins of early horse domestica- KI 5488: 4330 6 120 years B.P. (“ritual” skull) tion (Rassamakin 1994). These doubts Mean calibrated date: 2915 B.C. seem to be confirmed by the mean cali- 1 s; range 3092–2784 B.C. brated radiocarbon date recently ob- 3293 (0.03) 3277 3268 (0.05) 3240 3105 (0.75) 2865 tained for that skull, 2915 B.C., more 2809 (0.12) 2750 2724 (0.05) 2699 than 1000 years later than most of the 2 s; range 3347–2610 B.C. other dates for that site (Table 1) (Tele- 3339 (0.78) 2838 2828 (0.02) 2650 2650 (0.02) 2619 gin 1986). UCLA 1671A: 4900 6 100 years B.P. (bone) Mean calibrated dates: 3692, 3670 B.C. 1.2. METHODOLOGICAL PROBLEMS 1 s; range 3783–3548 B.C. 3892 (0.01) 3889 3796 (0.86) 2633 3577 (0.14) 3535 1.2.1. Conventional Approaches 2 s; range 3946–3383 B.C. 3946 (0.13) 3832 3829 (0.84) 3503 3417 (0.03) 3383 The theoretical framework used until recently for interpreting the archaeozoo- KI 2197: 5230 6 95 years B.P. (shell) logical data was seriously flawed (for a Mean calibrated dates: 4033, 4025, 3998, B.C. 1 s; range 4221–3959 B.C. more detailed discussion see Levine 1990, 4221 (0.11) 4193 4154 (0.89) 3959 1993). For example, the criteria used by 2 s; range 4320–3799 B.C. various researchers as evidence that the 4317 (0.02) 4292 4256 (0.89) 3902 3882 (0.09) 3802 horses from Dereivka were domesticated included the following: (1) the absence of OXA 5030: 5380 6 90 years B.P. (bone from old horses; (2) the presence of a large pro- cemetery) Mean calibrated date: 4237 B.C. portion of male skulls; (3) the presence of 1 s; range 4337–4048 B.C. objects identified as bridle cheekpieces; 4334 (0.58) 4216 4201 (0.28) 4141 4120 (0.14) 4087 (4) the results of a morphological analysis 2 s; range 4435–3985 B.C. comparing the Dereivka horses with other 4362 (1.00) 3988 equid material; (5) their association with other domesticates— cattle, sheep, goat, KI 2193: 5400 6 100 years B.P. (shell) pig, and dog; (6) the relatively large per- mean calibrated dates: 4310, 309, 4249 B.C. 1 s; range 4346–4086 B.C. centage of horse bones and teeth in the 4345 (0.63) 4216 4201 (0.25) 4141 4120 (0.12) 4087 deposit (Bökönyi 1978, 1984; Bibikova 2 s; range 4456–3985 B.C. 1967, 1970, 1969; Telegin 1986). However, 4451 (0.03) 4420 4396 (0.02) 4374 4369 (0.92) on the basis of archaeological, ethno- 4030 4030 (0.04) 3994 graphic, and ethological comparisons, the absence of old individuals is much more UCLA 1466a: 5515 6 90 BP (bone) Mean calibrated date: 4350 BC likely to indicate hunting than herding 1 sigma; Range 4457–4260 BC (Levine 1982, 1990). Males would outnum- 4458 (.83) 4317 4291 (0.17) 4256 ber females if either bachelor groups or 2 sigma; Range 4527–4155 BC stallions protecting their harems were tar- 4540 (.94) 4218 4198 (0.4) 4145 4115 (.01) 4093 geted in the hunt. The cheekpieces might not have been cheekpieces at all (Dietz Source. Dates from Telegin, personal communica- 1992; Levine and Rassamakin 1996). The tion, and conference abstract from Telegin (1995). Calibration from Stuiver and Reimer (1993). morphological study involved very small and disparate samples and produced con- tradictory results. The association of any case, they were also found with the horses with other assumed domesticates remains of wild animals (Levine 1990, is not evidence of horse domestication. In 1993). The only species from Dereivka to
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 33 be studied in detail was the horse. Be- 1.2.2. The Identification and Significance of cause almost all the bones and teeth from Bitwear this site have unfortunately been dis- carded, it is impossible for them to be As an analytical method, bitwear anal- reassessed. However, a preliminary exam- ysis should make a valuable contribution ination of the faunal assemblage from the to the study of horse domestication (An- new excavations at Molukhov Bugor, an- thony and Brown 1991). However, it has important limitations: other Dereivka culture site, 2 has produced some interesting, but extremely tentative 1. Tamed, as well as domesticated, results. No bones that could only have horses could wear bits. come from domesticated animals and 2. A horse can be ridden without a bit. many that must have come from wild 3. Anthony and Brown have themselves ones, for example, birds, tortoise, beaver, observed that bitwear traces will wear off deer, have been identified, while the cattle if a horse is not bitted regularly over a and pigs were suggestively enormous. relatively long period recently before its Much more work needs to be done on this death. assemblage, but the initial results lend 4. The question of whether the wear support to the far more detailed analyses pattern described by Anthony and Brown already carried out on the material from could have had other causes has not been adequately addressed. Their unbitted Dereivka (Levine 1990, 1993). On the one sample of feral horses consisted of 20 in- hand, there is little or no evidence that the dividuals from two North American pop- Dereivka culture people were pastoralists, ulations (mustangs from the mountains of while on the other hand, there is a good Nevada and barrier island ponies from reason to believe that they were hunter- the Atlantic Coast). They have generalized gatherers (Levine and Rassamakin 1996). from this small sample that unbitted Horses are relatively uncommon in horses could not manifest the wear pat- European Mesolithic and Neolithic ar- tern they describe as unique to bitwear. chaeological deposits. It has, therefore, On the other hand, Angela von den Dri- commonly been held to be the case that esch (personal communication) has ob- they could not have been domesticated served that similar, if not identical, wear during those periods. On the other on the lower second premolar can result hand, relatively large quantities of horse from abnormal occlusion with the upper bones and teeth have been recovered second premolar. from Eneolithic sites on the central Eur- As far as we know, then, beveling on the asian Steppe. Characteristics of tooth anterior part of the lower P2 masticatory morphology, population structure, ta- surface could be caused by bitwear or ab- phonomy, and taxonomic distinctions normal occlusion. Either a domesticated based on measurements, have been horse or a wild one that had been tamed credited as evidence for horse domesti- could be bitted. The absence of bitwear cation. Until recently, however, the most could indicate that a horse had not been important criterion had been that of in- ridden recently or regularly before its creased relative abundance, which could death, that it was ridden unbitted, or that be explained as well, or even better, by it never was ridden. We must conclude increased hunting rather than by from this that bitwear should not be used domestication (Bökönyi 1978, 1984; without corroboration as proof of domes- Bibikova 1967, 1970, 1969; Petrenko 1984; tication. This is not to say that bitwear Levine 1990, 1993). studies should not be carried out. On the
34 MARSHA A. LEVINE contrary, their use should be much more example, biomolecular analyses, stable widespread, but in conjunction with other isotope studies, paleopathology, ethnoar- methods of analysis. chaeology, ethology, and paleoenviron- ment research as well as more conven- 1.2.3. Sample Size and Innovation tional archaeological methods, is crucial to this approach. The goal of this paper is to Archaeologists and archaeozoologists take a step in that direction by using a continually lament the inadequacy of combination of ethnoarchaeological, etho- their samples. The assumption being logical, and archaeological analyses to that if only large enough datasets were look at the archaeological and archaeo- available, they would be able to find the zoological data. But this is only the begin- answer to any practically any question. ning. However, this might not be the case. Considering the skills needed for man- 2. POPULATION STRUCTURE AND aging large numbers of horses and con- MODELS OF HORSE EXPLOITATION sidering the small-scale nature of tam- ing, from which, as will be argued later, The particular aspect of horse hus- domestication is most likely to have bandry to be examined here is population evolved, the key to the origins of horse structure. Survivorship and mortality pat- domestication might well lie with small terns of recent horse herds are compared samples. Archaeozoologists must face with various models and with assem- up to this and develop methodologies blages from Eneolithic and Iron Age/Ro- that can cope with this reality. To regard man archaeological sites. The methodol- small samples only as a problem is to ogy used integrates taphonomy and miss an opportunity. butchery evidence with morphometrical, paleopathological, and population struc- 1.3. A MULTIDIMENSIONAL ture analyses. All of these are interpreted APPROACH with reference to ecological, ethological, ethnoarchaeological, and contextual data The common thread, connecting all as- (Levine 1979, 1982, 1983, 1990). pects of the project, out of which this pa- per has evolved is the question of the or- 2.1. Relationships between Horses igins and evolution of horse husbandry, and People its social and ecological implications— People can have a wide variety of dif- whether, for example, it arose out of agri- ferent types of relationships with horses. cultural, pastoral, or foraging communi- Horses can be wild, feral, 3 or domesti- ties— how the domestication of the horse cated. Wild or feral horses can be hunted altered the balance of power in ancient for their meat and other body parts, or communities, and its impact on forest– tamed as pets or beasts of burden. Do- steppe and steppe ecosystems. In the mesticated animals can be raised for broadest sense, my goal is to evaluate the riding, traction, meat, milk, and other ways in which environmental, social, and products. Moreover, even within one so- economic changes are interrelated and to ciety any combination of these relation- try to understand the role of the horse in ships can coexist. the equation. Such a complex problem Though customarily defined as the con- requires a multidimensional attack with trolled breeding of plants or animals by ammunition provided through the devel- humans, the real distinctiveness of do- opment of new analytical methods. Inter- mestication lies in the fact that it involves disciplinary collaboration, including, for ownership and thus results in a com-
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 35 pletely different level of human commit- have a good laugh. There is no reason why ment than does hunting (Levine 1979). the motivations of the informant should Horse taming also involves ownership, be any less complicated than those of the but it seems likely from the historical and interviewer. ethnographic evidence so far available 3. Which brings us to the distortions that the social and economic implications arising from the interviewer’s shortcom- of horse taming would have been, at most, ings. For example, phrasing a question relatively superficial and localized and clearly, but not leadingly, can be particu- would have disappeared with the death of larly difficult. Imperfect knowledge of the the animals involved, while the repercus- informant’s native language is a serious sions of domestication would have rever- problem. The horse husbandry and berated throughout the whole society. butchery vocabularies of most interpret- Our goal should not, therefore, be simply ers are not ideal. Moreover, it is impossi- to identify horse riding, traction, milking, ble to ask about everything. Certain limits and meat eating in the archaeological must be placed in respect for the time and record, but, additionally, to find evidence patience of the informant. Therefore, the of horse breeding and taming, which are, choice of which questions to ask is critical. as such, archaeologically invisible. How- They need to be unambiguous and di- ever, they may be approached indirectly rected specifically toward solving archae- through investigations of population struc- ological problems. ture, archaeological context, and other 4. Then, assuming that we have taken characteristics of the data. into account and minimized all these dif- Historical and ethnographic accounts, ficulties, we still have to deal with prob- as well as new ethnoarchaeological re- lems associated with the use and misuse search, are all employed here to gain ac- of ethnographic analogy, by its nature cess to that variability. However, it is im- highly complicated and potentially bi- portant to observe at the very outset of ased, to interpret the archaeological evi- this discussion that these kinds of data dence— equally complex and probably sources have their own particular prob- even more biased, for example, by tapho- lems. For example: nomic factors. 1. Inaccuracy. Particularly in the case of Archaeologists have been known to interviews relating to past practices, we throw up their arms in despair at the dif- can expect lapses of memory to distort ficulties encountered with ethnographic events that took place in the past. For ex- analogy and, indeed, some say that it can ample, in the case of interviews dealing only lead to tears. However, to interpret with the period before collectivization, 4 archaeological data we must have some my informants could not recall details of understanding of how human beings ac- herd population structure. tually behave. The unsatisfactory nature 2. Distortions resulting from the infor- of the work carried out until recently re- mant’s own personal agenda. It is well garding horse domestication has clearly known among anthropologists that infor- demonstrated this problem (Levine 1990, mants may have their own reasons for 1993). Without minimizing the difficulties what they say. For example, they may un- involved, it is therefore necessary to learn der- or overestimate the size of their how to use ethnographic and historical herds, if they think that there is an eco- data. Consequently, the objective of this nomic or political advantage to do so. ethnoarchaeological study is not the direct Moreover, many people will say what is interpretation of archaeological data from expected of them to please or simply to ethnographic and historical accounts, but
36 MARSHA A. LEVINE rather, an exploration of the range of ex- mounted on swift horses, and kill them tant possibilities, without assuming that with broad lances. Their flesh they esteem no others could have existed in the past. excellent food; and use their skins to sleep upon” (Mohr 1971, p. 27). 2.1.1. Capturing and Taming Horses According to 19th-century records, there were two methods of capturing Pr- According to Clutton-Brock, “A tame zewalski’s horse foals. One was to trap animal differs from a wild one in that it is them in pits dug near waterholes. The dependent on man and will stay close to other was for mounted men to chase and him of its own free will” (1987, p. 12). capture them with the arkan (a long pole Aboriginal hunter-gatherers and horticul- with a noose fastened to one end). When turists throughout the world are known to the pursuer came close enough to his tar- tame all kinds of wild animals to keep as get, he would drop the noose over its head pets (Serpell 1986, 1989). 5 There is no rea- and neck (Mohr 1971). Grum-Grshimailo son to think that this would not have been documents another method: “During the the case at least from the time of the ear- foaling season the Kalmucks take two liest anatomically modern Homo sapiens, horses into the desert. As soon as they and when the need arose, taming could have found a herd, they chase them until well have been the first step toward do- the exhausted foals fall over. These foals mestication (Galton 1883; Clutton-Brock are picked up and placed in the domesti- 1987; Serpell 1989). Wild horses, particu- cated herd” (Mohr 1971, p. 68). Przewals- larly as foals, can be captured and tamed ki’s horses were also captured by driving, and, as such, ridden or harnessed and, at though it is not clear whether the beaters the end of their lives, if necessary, slaugh- were on foot or horseback: “Even in 1750 it tered and eaten. was said: ‘The entire land around Lyau- 2.1.1.1. Taming the Przewalski’s horse. His- tong is a wilderness; the emperor hunts torical records also show that the capture, there with three thousand beaters, who taming, and eventual captive breeding of put up the game and drive it towards him, wild horses was dependent on the accu- so that in one day 200 to 300 horses, mulation of knowledge about their behav- amongst others may be caught’” (Mohr ior and on the development of techniques 1971, p. 27). to exploit that behavior. Perhaps the ear- The early 20th-century collectors found liest record of a horse captured by these that their greatest difficulty was not in means dates from 113 B.C.: catching the horses, but rather in keeping A Chinese . . . near Tun-huang, on the north- them alive in captivity. Attempts to feed west frontier, frequently saw a horse . . . drink- unweaned foals on sheep and goat milk ing in the river along with a number of wild were not successful. The solution to this horses. He tamed the strange horse by putting at problem was to foster them with domes- the water-side a dummy figure of a man in ticated mares (Bouman and Bouman whose hands were bridle and halter. When the horse was used to this sight he substituted him- 1994). According to Frederick von Falz- self for the dummy, captured the horse. (Waley Fein, one of the early collectors: 1955, pp. 98 –9) In 1897 a number of young wild horses were In another example Mohr refers to the captured, but they all died because the catch description by John Bell, an 18th-century was not done as it should have been. I worked out the fullest details of the method and laid Scottish doctor and traveler, of Przewals- much stress on the importance of the animals ki’s horse hunting from horseback: “these not being chased before capture, but rather by animals are often surprised by the Kal- shooting their mothers. As we could not get mucks; who ride in among them, well milking mares from the Mongolians living in the
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 37 area, we had to buy them in Bijsk, and have old son . . . to sit on his back” (Mohr 1971, them covered so that they foaled at the same p. 69). She also describes how an “untam- time as the wild mares.. . . Since these rules were not obeyed—the catch was again unsuccessful able” wild stallion was tamed and ridden: and all the animals died. We told Assanoff again “In Askania Nova however, he found his to stick to the rules and thereafter there were no master and within a month he was being more failures. (Mohr 1971, 95–96) ridden by his south Russian groom and on the command would lie down like a Cir- Przewalski’s horse mares currently in cassian horse” (Mohr 1971, p. 69). captivity usually wean their young from This has important implications for the- the age of 1 year until just before the birth ories concerning early horse taming. It of their next foal, or even for several years seems likely that before the availability of if they do not give birth every year (Houpt domesticated mares to foster captured and Boyd 1994). Berger observed of the foals, there would have been both lower Great Basin feral horses that 34 of 40 (85%) and upper limits to the ages at which tam- were weaned before the age of 1 year and ing would have been successful. Although 27 (79%) of those were not observed to very few data relevant to this question suckle after their ninth month. “Because seem to be available, the lower limit might of winter-related stresses and because the have been at around the age of 2 months. last trimester of pregnancy demands the We can only speculate about a possible most nutritionally . . . mothers weaned their upper limit on the basis of comments in offspring during winters” (Berger 1986, p. the literature referring to the difficulty or 116). Foals can be weaned much earlier. impossibility of taming adults (Mohr 1971). However, there is a cost to pay: “Evidence However, other factors, which would also for the importance of milk versus highly have been critical, include the skills of the nutritious food for early growth rates is captor and the personality of the horse. still sparse, but animal scientists have 2.1.1.2. Taming North American feral found that orphaned foals experience horses. Some parallels between central stunted development despite provision- Eurasian and North American aboriginal ing with high planes of nutrition” (Berger, horse capture and taming techniques are 1986, p. 119). Berger mentions a mustang particularly interesting because they sug- from the Granite Range (Nevada), or- gest that certain aspects of the human– phaned at the age of 2 months. Despite horse relationship are not culture-bound, access to good-quality grazing, even at the but are rather mediated by both species’ age of 3 years, he was only the size of a natural patterns of behavior in a much yearling. Similarly a captive Przewalski more fundamental way. For example, ac- foal, orphaned at the age of 221 months, cording to Ewers (1955), northern Plains survived but lagged in growth behind his peoples such as the Blackfoot and the unorphaned paternal half-siblings until Cree were not very skilled at taming mus- the age of 3 years despite supplemental tangs, the North American feral horses. feeding (Houpt and Boyd 1994). Most of the few adult feral horses cap- Taming and riding Przewalski’s horses tured by them died after they reached captured from the wild was at one time camp. However, some colts and yearlings considered to be practically impossible were caught by “horse medicine men,” (Mohr 1971). However, Erna Mohr refers specialist feral horse tamers, whose tam- to a 6-month-old Przewalski horse that ing technique was described as follows: “had become so far tame that it was easily A man who possessed horse medicine for use in led and went quietly up the granite stair- catching wild horses rubbed it on his hands, feet, case to the second story of the castle, was and rope. Then he circled the wild horse up wind led into a room and allowed the 7– 8 year so that the odour of the medicine would be carried
38 MARSHA A. LEVINE to the nostrils of the wild one. When the wild tion of a corral apparently used by the horse smelled the medicine it came to him. He Cheyenne: roped it by the front feet and threw it down. Only horse medicine men were said to have had success [I]n the year 1836, members of Cheyenne war in capturing wild horses. (Ewers 1955, p. 274) parties . . . in what is now Oklahoma, found a great corral which had been used for catching According to Ewers, the southern and horses. This pen was situated in a park or open- central Plains tribes were much more ing in the black-jack timber . . . This pen was not circular in shape, but was oval, the opening be- skilled than the northern tribes at captur- ing at one end. The fence . . . was a stockade ing mustangs. The former had more and formed of black-jack posts set on end in the earlier experience of horses and they had ground and close together. On the outside of the bigger herds, which suggests that they fence brush and the limbs of trees were piled were more familiar with horse behavior. against the stockade. The wings of underbrush were heaped up high and wide, so that a horse However, all the Plains groups were in could neither see through nor jump over agreement that mustangs were difficult to them . . . catch. According to George Catlin: . . . the Kiowas explained to the Cheyennes the purpose and the manner of use of the structure. There is no other animal on the prairies so wild Of the horses driven into this corral the best and so sagacious as the horse; and none other so young ones were roped and dragged out to be difficult to come up with. So remarkably keen is used, while the older and otherwise less useful their eye, that they will generally run “at the animals were butchered for their flesh and sight,” when they are a mile distant; being, no hides. The Kiowas used horse-hide for all pur- doubt, able to distinguish the character of the poses for which the skins of large animals are enemy that is approaching when at that dis- employed. (Grinnell 1923, p. 292) tance; and when in motion, will seldom stop short of three or four miles. (Catlin 1841, Vol. 1, 2. The Chase: All other things being p. 57). equal a man on horseback is no match for The two main tools used for capturing a free-running mustang. Therefore, the feral horses were the lasso with a running Indians developed variations on the chase loop and the lasso loop fixed to a long theme that would enable them to capture stick, very much like the Mongol arkan strong, healthy animals. (Ewers 1955). In conjunction with an inti- a. Chasing animals in a weakened mate knowledge of horse behavior and a state: The ethnographic literature is not fit, well-trained mount, these could be always clear about details of how horses used successfully to capture and break were captured. However, the most wide- mustangs (Catlin 1841; Ewers 1955; Grin- spread method seems to have involved nell 1923; James 1823; Wallace and Hoebel running down the mustangs on horseback 1952). A number of methods of capturing and dropping a noose over their head. horses have been described in the ethno- This method was employed by the Man- graphic literature: dan and the Osage (Catlin 1841), the Com- 1. Corralling: This method was used ex- manche (Wallace and Hoebel 1952), and tensively by the Kiowa and occasionally the Cheyenne (Grinnell 1923). As ob- by the Commanche 6 and Cheyenne. Wal- served by Wallace and Hoebel, all other lace and Hoebel (1952) speculate that it things being equal, this method could be could have evolved out of antelope and successful only for weak animals, for ex- bison hunts, but the same technique was ample, foals and pregnant or suckling also used by the Spanish for hunting mares, since a strong horse should be able horses. On one occasion in 1852, 400 to 500 to run faster than a horse and rider. How- horses were driven into an enclosure by ever, things were not always equal. For the Commanches (Wallace and Hoebel example, using a well-fed riding horse 1952). Grinnell gives a detailed descrip- gave the captor an advantage in the winter
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 39 or early spring when most feral horses dency to circle to the left when being fol- were in poor condition. On the other lowed: hand, a fit horse could be used to run The Indian seeing the direction in which the down fat horses in summer or to chase horse is “leaning,” knows just about the point “waterlogged” horses just leaving the wa- where the animal will stop, and steers in a terhole (Wallace and Hoebel 1952; Grin- straight line to it, where they arrive nearly at the same instant, the horse having run a mile, while nell 1923). his pursuer has gone but half or three-quarters b. Chasing in relays: This method was of the distance. (Catlin 1875, p. 113). expensive in terms of energy consump- He would thus keep it on the move until it tion, but would have resulted in the cap- was so exhausted that he could throw a ture of the very best animals, including lasso over its head. stallions. It was used by the Osage (James 3. Capturing with decoys: Both the 1823) and the Commanche (Wallace and Cheyenne and the Commanches targeted Hoebel 1952). bachelor groups by sending out a few old, In capturing the hard-to-take stallions . . . the gentle mares as decoys (Wallace and best way was to stalk them with a team of co- Hoebel 1952; Grinnell, 1923). According to operating hunters. Each herd tended to move Grinnell, “after a time the herd could be about within a limited range of territory; when approached, driven together, and perhaps flushed, it was likely to travel in a circle, return- ing eventually to or near the spot where it was many of the young horses caught” (Grin- originally found. To accomplish this end, one or nell 1923, p. 295). more horsemen kept the herd continuously on 2.1.1.3. Taming captured mustangs. Some the move without allowing it either to eat or to drink . . . the stalkers, by remaining on the in- of the ethnographic reports are rather side of the circle, travelled a much shorter dis- self-contradictory in that they suggest, on tance than the herd. When their own mounts the one hand, that taming feral horses was wearied, the riders were replaced by others or very difficult while, on the other hand, were supplied with fresh mounts. This proce- they describe the process as if it were very dure was continued without let-up for two or simple. For example, regarding the ab- three days or until the herd became exhausted, when a number of riders on fresh mounts rode original inhabitants of the Great Plains, in and lassoed their pick of the wild horses.” Catlin states that “Scarcely a man in these (Wallace and Hoebel 1952, p. 44) regions is to be found, who is not the owner of one or more of these horses; and c. The surround: On the open plains, in many instances of eight, ten or even mustangs would be surrounded by a twenty, which he values as his own per- group of riders. When a horse would try to sonal property” (Catlin 1841a, p. 142). break away, a noose was dropped over its Moreover, with regard to the acquisition head (Wallace and Hoebel 1952). of a mustang by a Frenchman, raised in an Osage village, he remarks: “the whole d. Chasing on foot: According to Catlin, thing, the capture, and breaking, all hav- the Cheyenne, who captured more mus- ing been accomplished within the space of tangs than any other tribe, frequently one hour, our usual and daily halt at mid- used this method. A horseman would day” (Catlin 1841b, p. 60). This paradox is start out by “plunging” into a band of wild partly explained by the diverse origins of horses, forcing one animal out of the the sources referred to here, but perhaps group, whereupon he would dismount also by the talent that experts have to from his own animal and set out on foot make the most difficult activities appear after the panicked individual. This is an- simple. In other words, it is possible that other method that exploits the horse’s ten- the European observers overstated their
40 MARSHA A. LEVINE understanding of the events taking place Interestingly this method employs the around them. This ignorance is well illus- same kind of psychological approach as trated by Catlin’s account of his own mis- that recently developed by Monty Robert begotten attempt to capture a feral horse: in which the safe space or “comfort zone,” occupied by the gentle but dominant [W]e would try the experiment of “creasing” trainer, is opposed to the dangerous space one . . . which is done by shooting them through the gristle on the top of the neck, which stuns away from him, in which the horse feels them so that they fall, and are secured with threatened and isolated (Bayley and Max- hobbles on the feet; after which they rise again well 1996). This training method takes ad- without fatal injury. This is a practice often re- vantage both of the horse’s instinctive sorted to by expert hunters. . . . My friend Joe flight response and of its natural sociabil- and I . . . having both levelled our pieces at the ity. withers of a noble, fine-looking iron grey, we pulled trigger, and the poor creature fell. . . .We One Commanche and Cheyenne method advanced speedily to him, and had the most of taming involved tying the choked cap- inexpressible mortification . . . to find that one of tive to the tail of a gentle mare (Wallace our shots had broken the poor creature’s neck, and Hoebel 1952; Grinnell 1923): and that he was quite dead. (Catlin 1841b, p. 58) Three or four days later . . . it was set free, and Despite its shortcomings, it is useful to thereafter followed her about wherever she went. The mare was then used to tame another consider some of the documentation re- horse, and if the party was out for a long time ferring to the process of breaking and some mares might have eight or ten captured taming feral horses. According to Catlin, horses following them about. These wild horses the affect of the lasso on the horse was to were readily broken to the saddle. While they constrict its air passage until it fell over, were “tailed” to the mare, the owner would oc- casionally go up to the mare, pat her for a little whereupon its captor hobbled its forefeet while, and then pass on to the young horse, together, fitted a halter with a noose that handling it and gentling it. In this way it became tied under its jaw, and loosened the lasso accustomed to the sight and smell of man, and so that it could breathe. Then, no longer feared him. Sometimes after the horse had become somewhat gentle, a young man by a great many useless struggles to rise, the would spring on its back and at once jump off horse remaining yet in its sitting posture, and again. The wild horse soon learned that it was the Indian approaching nearer and nearer (inch not to be hurt. The man who mounted would by inch) to its nose, on the shortened halter, and presently sit on the horse for a little while, and yelling as loud as he can, the animal’s fear is then the old mare might be led about by some- increased to the highest degree. The Indian still one while the young man was sitting on the wild advances nearer on the tightened halter, and at horse’s back. Thus the work of breaking it to ride length begins patting the horse on the nose, and was not long. (Grinnell 1923, pp. 294 –295) gradually slipping his hand over its eyes, begins breathing in its nostrils, their noses being to- Unfortunately, no survival rates are gether. available for any of these methods. But After a few breaths exchanged in this manner, some were, apparently, brutal enough to the relaxation of the horse’s muscles and its explain the difficulty some groups experi- other motions, show that its fears are at an end— enced in keeping captives alive. Other rel- that it recognises a friend instead of a foe, in its evant factors could well have been the captor; and this compromise being effected, the Indian is seen stroking down its mane, and oth- age, sex, constitution, and personality of erwise caressing it; and in fifteen or twenty min- the horse as well as the skill of the captor. utes he is seen riding it quietly off! . . . the excess of fatigue, of fright, and actual 2.1.2. Modelling Horse Use pain, followed by soothing and kindness, seems to disarm the spirited animal, and to attach it at once, in a mysterious way, to its new master. During historical times both the North (Catlin 1875, p. 109 –110) American Plains tribes and the Mongols
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 41 used the arkan, lasso, or herd drive to cap- age. The first mares placed with the stallion ture wild or feral horses to eat or to tame. should be younger than he and the harem size should be kept small until the stallion gains age Horses taming was regarded as a skill and experience. (Boyd and Houpt 1994, p. 226) most successfully carried out by special- ists, whose most important tool was their That capturing wild horses and stealing intimate knowledge of horse behavior. On tamed or domesticated ones were re- this basis I would like to propose a possi- garded by the Plains tribes as preferable ble scenario for the development of horse to breeding them supports the scenario husbandry. proposed here. If it is correct, it seems As a working hypothesis, I would like to likely that there would have been a rela- suggest that horse taming probably first tively long period when new horses would arose as a by-product of horse hunting for have been recruited from wild popula- meat. Orphaned foals, captured between tions. This could have been carried out by the ages of perhaps 2 months and 1 year, trapping, driving, and chasing, as docu- or possibly somewhat later, would some- mented for the Mongols and North Amer- times have been adopted and raised as ican Plains tribes. pets. Eventually, and perhaps repeatedly, This leads me to hypothesize that horse the discovery was made that these pets domestication could have taken a rela- could be put to work. This knowledge tively long time to develop and might well could have been acquired and lost many have depended on the taming of individ- times from the Pleistocene onward. But it uals predisposed to breed in captivity. was, apparently, only during the Holo- Horse domestication would thus, in a cene—possibly between the Neolithic and sense, have been initiated by the horses the Early Bronze Age—that it began to themselves. Also significant is the possi- influence human social developments. bility that human understanding of horse Initially the difficulties involved in behavior had developed to such a degree keeping captured wild horses alive would that horses finally could breed in captiv- have set limits to their impact as work ity. Perhaps the most likely scenario is animals on human society. Furthermore, that the human and equine parts of the considering the problems encountered by equation would have evolved together. modern collectors trying to breed Przew- The development of horse breeding alski’s horses, it seems likely that horse- would, of course, have had particular sig- keeping would have had to have been nificance outside the natural range of the relatively advanced before controlled wild horse. breeding, and thus domestication, would 2.2. An Ethnoarchaeological have been possible: “Failure to consider Investigation of Equine Pastoralism the typical social organization of the spe- cies can result in problems such as pacing, Scholars from Russia and other parts of excessive rates of aggression, impotence Eastern Europe have carried out im- and infanticide” (Boyd and Houpt 1994, p. mensely valuable ethnographic research 222). To breed wild horses successfully in on central Eurasian equine pastoralism. captivity, their environmental, nutritional, However, this work does not usually di- and social requirements must be met: rectly address the questions of particular relevance to the study of the origins of In zoos, juvenile male Przewalski’s horses horse domestication. The project to be should be left in their natal bands for at least a discussed below has been designed spe- year so that they can observe mating behaviour. They should be placed in bachelor herds when cifically to deal with issues connected with removed from the natural band, and not given that problem. It presents some results harems until they are at least four or five years of from an ongoing ethnoarchaeological
42 MARSHA A. LEVINE TABLE 2 Informant’s Background Information Husbandry Economic Informant Location Ecosystem type system Damdin E. Mongolia Steppe Traditional Nomadic Jambalsuren C. Mongolia Mountains Traditional Settled Mursabaev N. Kazakhstan Forest–steppe Modern Ranching Shavardak N. Kazakhstan Forest–steppe Modern Settled Kozakhmetov N. Kazakhstan Forest–steppe Traditional Semi-nomadic study of equine pastoralism on the Eur- riod discussed here will be that of their asian steppe. The data have arisen princi- childhood or as far back as their parents’ pally in the course of five interviews, con- reminiscences. Thus, traditional, as de- ducted between 1989 and 1992, with fined here, extends from the end of the people involved with horse husbandry in 19th century to the 1930s in the case of Mongolia and northern Kazakhstan in the Kazakhstan and to the 1950s in the case of recent past or present. Mongolia. 2.2.1.1. Background information (Table 2.2.1. The Interviews 2). The first two interviews were carried out in Cambridge in 1989 and 1990. The Although the interviews covered all as- informants, Damdin and Jambalsuren, were pects of horse husbandry—from those visiting scholars at the Mongolian and In- related to riding and traction to those con- ner Asian Studies Unit (Cambridge). nected with milk, meat, and hide produc- Damdin, a senior lecturer in the Depart- tion—this paper concentrates on those ment of Foreign Languages (Ulan Bator associated with population structure. It at- University, Mongolia), grew up on the tempts to demonstrate how certain ar- steppe in the extreme eastern part of chaeozoologically visible characteristics of Mongolia, in the Jargalant district of the horse husbandry, such as age and sex Dornod province during the late 1930s structure, fit into the overall picture of and 1940s. He was from a family of pasto- pastoral life. It also gives some indication ral nomads belonging to the Khalkha clan. of the variability of possible behaviors re- Since collectivization did not take place in lated to equine pastoralism. Although the Dornod until 1955, the way of life he de- data collected are not generally appropri- scribes was still rather traditional. Impor- ate for direct translation into life tables, tant characteristics of this lifestyle include they can be used for the development of the absence of permanent dwellings (they models and in general comparisons. lived in felt tents, known as yurts), and The word traditional is used here pri- year-round migrations, seasonal in char- marily to describe precollectivization acter, in search of grazing for their herds methods of horse husbandry. Collectiviza- of horses, sheep, goats, cattle, and camels. tion took place— or perhaps more signifi- Jambalsuren (Academy of Sciences, In- cantly, took hold—in different places at stitute of Language, Ulan Bator, Mongo- different times. In northern Kazakhstan it lia) grew up in the mountainous region of is dated to the 1930s, but it was not im- central Mongolia during the 1950s. His fa- posed on Mongolia until 1955. Since my ther was a carpenter and his family was informants’ accounts are entirely depen- settled. Until the age of 16 years Jambal- dent on their memories, the earliest pe- suren was a yak herder. Because his father
ORIGINS OF HORSE DOMESTICATION 43 was a craftsman and because of the moun- TABLE 3 tainous terrain, his family did not have Number of Horses per Household many horses. He, therefore, had only a Informant Minimum Average Maximum limited knowledge of horse husbandry. The next three interviews took place in Damdin 10 20–301 100s–1000s 1992 in the forest–steppe zone of northern Jambalsuren 1/person 40 3000 Mursabaev 1 — — Kazakhstan, where the informants live. Shavardak 1 — 10 Dastan Chalievich Murzabaev, president Kozhakhmetov 4–10 40 300 of the trade union at the Kirov sovkhoz in the Dzhambul region of North Kazakhstan, discussed contemporary horse ranching at the state farm where he worked. (steppe and forest–steppe) and husbandry Yurii Ivanovich Shavardak works for strategies (nomadic, semi-nomadic, and the Burlukskii sovkhoz (Volodarovskii settled) are represented in these inter- views. The diversity of the data gives district, North Kazakhstan). He herds all some idea of the range of possible strate- the horses from Nikolskoe, a village near gies available to horse herders in the the archaeological site of Botai. Most of steppe and forest–steppe regions of cen- the horses are owned privately by the Ka- tral Eurasia. It is hypothesized that simi- zakh inhabitants of the village, but some larities and differences arising from that belong to the sovkhoz. Shavardak grew up diversity might have some value in eluci- in Nikolskoe and, although he is Russian, dating some of the fundamental elements he was trained to herd and butcher horses of equine pastoralism. Of course, care using a mixture of modern, that is, post- must be taken in generalizing from such a collectivization, and traditional Kazakh small sample. Moreover, throughout the methods. Collectivization took place in millennia waves of change have repeat- this region during the 1930s. edly swept across Eurasia, drawing people Mamet Kozhakhmetovich Kozhakhme- together and tearing them apart, remind- tov, born in 1915, is a former herdsmen, ing us that history is no bit player in this then schoolteacher, and finally, at the time story. of the interview, a pensioner. He was born 2.2.1.2. Number of horses per household and brought up at Botai aul 7 (Karatalskii (Table 3). Generalizations about the sovkhoz, Volodarovskii district, North Ka- quantity of horses in settled households zakhstan). With the help of Eslyambey are not very useful, but it is quite interest- Zakir’yanovich Zakir’yanov, his relative ing to compare figures obtained from and headmaster of the school in Nikols- Damdin and Kozhakhmetov concerning koe, he described horse husbandry as it the period before collectivization. Taking was in his childhood, before collectiviza- care not to read too much into a sample of tion. The people from Botai aul are per- 2, it does seem that concepts relating to manently settled now, but before collec- herd size were very similar for both the tivization they were seminomadic. They nomadic Mongols and semi-nomadic Ka- spent the cold months of the year in the zakhs interviewed. To carry out seasonal permanent dwellings of the aul. In the migrations at least 10 horses were neces- summer, however, they moved onto the sary. An average household had about 20 steppe. Each household had traditional to 40 and a rich household might have rights to a particular territory and to a plot kept hundreds or even thousands of of land where they could set their yurt horses. These figures are in line with each year. those given by Khazanov (1984), Tokta- A relatively wide variety of ecosystems baev (1992), and Krader (1955). Shavard-
44 MARSHA A. LEVINE TABLE 4 Herd Population Structure Herd sex composition Informant Reproductive unit Gelding structure Stallions Mares Foals Geldings Damdin Family group With family group 1 15–20 15–20 15–20 Jambalsuren Family group Near family group 1 15 15 10 (0) a (1) (0) (4) Mursabaev Stallions 1 mares In separate group 1 25 ? ? Shavardak Family group In separate group 1 45 45 15 Kozhakhmetov Family group In separate group 1 15–20 20 ? a Figures in parentheses refer to his own family’s horses. ak’s herd comprises about 100 horses, ploit to some extent the natural tendency around 15 of which belong to the state of horses to structure themselves into farm, while the rest are privately owned. family groups. That is, the pastoralist re- Nearly all the Kazakh households in Ni- productive unit mimics the natural family kolskoe have at least one horse, while group, composed of a stallion, his mares, some have as many as 10. and their young. However, the structure 2.2.1.3. Population structure (Table 4). The of the pastoralist herd is, in all cases, dis- natural reproductive unit of the torted by the artificially large number of horse is the family group, composed of a mares assigned to each stallion. This is stallion, his mares, and their young up to most extreme for the nontraditional herd- the age of about 2 to 4 years. It may com- ers. The ratio of 1 stallion to 15 to 20 mares prise up to 21 mares, although the average is remarkably constant in the traditional is usually much less, perhaps around 2 to context. This is particularly interesting in 4 and usually no more than 5 or 6 (Berger the light of an observation by Houpt and 1986; Klingel 1969, 1974; Bouman and Bou- Boyd that “Przewalski’s stallions with har- man 1994, Houpt and Boyd 1994). The stal- ems of thirteen to eighteen females have lion normally starts his own family group become overly aggressive toward their at the age of 5 or 6 years, although he mares or apathetic about breeding” (Boyd might not be successful at holding one and Houpt 1994, p. 226). That the domestic against attacks from other males until the mare:stallion ratio is only a little greater age of 7 (Klingel 1969; Berger 1986; Mon- than the Przewalski ratio, attests both to fort et al. 1994; Houpt and Boyd 1994). The the consistency of horse behavior and to second natural type of horse social unit is the herders’ knowledge. Geldings are the the bachelor group, made up entirely of domestic equivalent of equine bachelors. males from the age of 2 years until their All males surplus to breeding require- departure from the group to form their ments are castrated. own bands and, less commonly, of older In the Mongolian cases all age and sex males who have lost theirs to stronger classes graze more or less together. In stallions. The bachelor group may com- Damdin’s pastoral nomadic example, the prise up to 15 individuals, but the average geldings graze in their natal family is much lower, about 2 to 4 (Klingel 1969; groups. According to Jambalsuren, geld- Berger 1986). ings graze together near the family group The structure of the wild herd is rele- but apart from it. The Kazakh herd struc- vant here because all the horse husbandry ture seems generally to be more compli- patterns, described by my informants, ex- cated. According to Murzabaev’s ranching
You can also read