The Journal of Emergent Science - whyhh & howoo ?w - Association for Science Education
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Contents Issue 15 Summer 2018 Editorial Contributions Editors: Amanda McCrory 3. Editorial Suzanne Gatt Executive Editor: Jane Hanrott janehanrott@ase.org.uk 2017 ESERA Conference special issue Cover Photo Courtesy of: iStock.com/DGLimages 4. Guest editorial. Coral Campbell (see article on page 13) 6. Systemising and empathising in early years science – a videobased study with preschool children. Nina Skorsetz and Manuela WelzelBreuer Publisher: Association for Science 13. Preschool children’s collaborative science learning scaffolded by tablets – Education (ASE) College Lane, a teacher’s view. Marie Fridberg, Susanne Thulin and Andreas Redfors Hatfield, Herts, AL10 9AA, UK 20. Potential for multidimensional teaching for ‘emergent scientific literacy’ in ©ASE 2018 preschool practice. Sofie Areljung and Bodil Sundberg ISSN: 20464754 28. Reflections on guidance to orientate untrained practitioners towards authentic science for children in the early years. Linda McGuigan and Terry Russell The Journal of Emergent Science (JES) is published 37. Teaching science in Australian bush kindergartens – understanding what by ASE in partnership with teachers need. Coral Campbell and Christopher Speldewinde the Primary Science Teaching Trust (PSTT). It is free to access for all. Primary Science Teaching Trust (PSTT) 46. Reflections on and analysis of the use of drama techniques and dialogic practices in teaching science in a primary school. Clarysly Deller 57. Teachers’ attempts to improve assessment practice in primary science are influenced by job role and teaching experience. Isabel HopwoodStephens 67. News from the Primary Science Teaching Trust (PSTT) Regular features 68. Guidelines for authors why h & 70. About ASE how ow? PRIMARY SCIENCE TEACHING TRUST
Editorial l Amanda McCrory l Suzanne Gatt For this Special Issue of JES, we are very fortunate primary classroom. The first, written by Clarysly to have Dr. Coral Campbell from Deakin University Deller, reflects on how drama techniques and in Geelong, Australia as a guest editor, who dialogic practices can be used to effectively teach explains – further on in her own Editorial – the aspects of primary science. The second, by Isabel importance of the focus of this issue – the 2017 HopwoodStephens, grapples with the tensions ESERA Conference. Coral has been involved in that classroom practitioners face when attempting industry, school and tertiary education for many to improve assessment practice in primary science. years, and her research interests lie in practitioner Both articles make for thoughtprovoking reading! learning and students’ understanding in science and STEM, in all areas of the schooling sector. Her We very much hope that you enjoy the varied and more recent research has focused on early engaging articles presented in this issue of JES. childhood STEM. We would like to thank ESERA for their kind permission to publish the extended abstracts of some of the 2017 Conference contributions in this edition. In particular, we are very grateful to Professor Costas Constantinou, President of Amanda McCrory, Institute of Education, ESERA, for his support. We will publish a second University College of London collection in issue 16. Email: a.mccrory@ucl.ac.uk Suzanne Gatt, Faculty of Education, In addition, this issue includes two articles from the University of Malta Primary Science Teaching Trust (PSTT), both of Email: suzanne.gatt@um.edu.mt which give interesting insights into science in the CoEditors of the Journal of Emergent Science Editorial JES14 Winter 2017/18 page 3
Guest Editorial l Coral Campbell Welcome to this edition of the Journal of Emergent In this edition of JES, we have included the Science (JES), which highlights some very interesting extended abstracts from some of the presentations research through some short papers presented at the delivered in Dublin. Obviously, we could not publish 2017 European Science Education Research everything, but feel that these offer a ‘taster’ of the Association (ESERA) Conference in Dublin, Ireland. international research happening across the globe. It is intended to include some more of these Firstly, let me explain that the two JES Editors have abstracts in issue 16 of JES, later in the year. Also, asked me to write this Editorial, as I was the person if you would like further examples, please refer to who approached them about this special edition. the ESERA website (https://keynote.conference This was as a consequence of my role as ESERA services.net/programme.asp?conferenceID=5233). Early Childhood (EC) Science Special Interest Group Contributors to this edition include: (SIG) Coordinator, a role shared with Estelle Blanquet. At the ESERA Conference in 2017, the p Reflections on guidance to orientate untrained SIG discussed ways to improve the visibility and practitioners towards authentic science for impact of early childhood science. Jane Johnston children in the early years – Linda Mcguigan and and Lady Sue Dale Tunnicliffe were the original Terry Russell. Editors of JES as well as the first ESERA EC Science p Potential for multidimensional teaching for SIG coordinators, so it seemed fitting that ESERA ‘emergent scientific literacy’ in preschool and JES were associated again through this edition. practice – Sofie Areljung and Bodil Sundberg. p Preschool children’s collaborative science At the recent Conference in Ireland, there was a learning scaffolded by tablets – a teacher’s view – significant increase in the number and type of Marie Fridberg, Susanne Thulin and Andreas papers submitted around topics of research into Redfors. early years science education. For example, when the SIG started (2009), there were approximately p Teaching science in Australian bush 15 people who submitted papers in two sessions. In kindergartens – understanding what teachers 2011, in Lyon, there were three sessions and 19 need – Coral Campbell and Christopher papers presented, as well as quite a few poster Speldewinde. presentations, each with a standard synopsis paper p Systemising and empathising in early years of the research. In 2017, the number of papers science – a videobased study with preschool presented through six paper presentation sessions, children – Nina Skorsetz and Manuela two symposium sessions and one poster session, WelzelBreuer. had increased to 35. I attended as many sessions as I could, and enjoyed the variety of research being undertaken and the The indications, from the increasing number of robust discussions afterwards. Interestingly, submissions to the conferences, imply that early I attended the European Early Childhood Education childhood science education is receiving more Research Conference a short time after ESERA and notice in the science education research found yet more early childhood science papers community and is attracting more researchers presented there. There were significantly fewer of looking at the depth and breadth of science them, but still of excellent quality and, of course, education provision in early childhood. discussion was engaging. Guest Editorial JES15 Summer 2018 page 4
I invite you to sit back, read and enjoy the content of this edition of JES. If a particular article intrigues you, please follow it up with the author – it is likely that further research has been published in the last few months. Coral Campbell Dr. Coral Campbell is an Associate Professor in science education at Deakin University in Victoria, Australia. Her research interests include early childhood and primary science/STEM education. Guest Editorial JES15 Summer 2018 page 5
Systemising and empathising in early years science – a video-based study with pre-school children l Nina Skorsetz l Manuela WelzelBreuer research approach (Collective, 2003), the tested This paper has also appeared in: Finlayson, O., children were videoobserved while participating McLoughlin, E., Erduran, S. & Childs, P. (Eds.) in the two different scientific learning environments, (2018) Electronic Proceedings of the ESERA 2017 in spring 2015 and 2016. Conference. Research, Practice and Collaboration in Science Education. Dublin, Results seem to show that children with a high Ireland: Dublin City University. SQvalue, as reported in literature, tend to be more ISBN 9781873769843. motivated to do science than children with a high Reproduced here with permission from ESERA. EQvalue. Children with a high SQvalue were motivated in both learning environments, which could lead to the interpretation that these children Abstract are motivated to do science independent from the Children are very different in their motivation to do pedagogical arrangement of the learning science. An approach used to explain these environment. For children with a high EQvalue, differences in the motivation for science could be no such correlations for their motivation to do through the EmpathizingSystemizing (ES)Theory science were found. They seem to be less motivated (BaronCohen, 2009). This theory states that every in both learning environments than children with person’s brain has two dimensions: the systemising high SQvalue. More research is needed. and the empathising. Both dimensions can be measured with a questionnaire and represented in an Keywords: Early years science, videobased EQ and a SQvalue. research, designbased research People with a high SQvalue are called ‘systemisers’ Introduction and tend to search for systems behind things; Starting point: Diversity ‘empathisers’ orientate themselves to other people’s Children in kindergarten are often very motivated feelings. Systemisers are generally more engaged in to do science, but this motivation varies from child science and more motivated to do science than to child and fades away with age (Patrick & people with a high EQvalue, who are stronger in Mantzicopoulos, 2015). One usual explanation for empathising (Zeyer et al, 2013). the different motivation to do science is the gender difference between boys and girls. A slightly The main goal of this study is to find out if preschool different approach for explaining differences in the children with various EQ and SQvalues act motivation for science is the Empathizing differently in different scientific learning Systemizing (ES)Theory by BaronCohen (2002). environments. Children were observed during two The basis of his theory is that every human brain pedagogically differently arranged learning has two dimensions. On the one hand, there is the environments, to investigate potential different ‘empathising’ dimension, which is defined by the behaviour. In this study, the brain types with respect drive to ‘identify another‘s mental states and to to the EQ and SQvalues of 4 to 6 yearold pre respond to these with an appropriate emotion, in school children were determined with a 55item EQ order to predict and to respond to the behaviour of SQ questionnaire (Auyeung et al, 2009), which was another person’ (BaronCohen et al, 2005). On the translated into German. In terms of a designbased other hand, there is the ‘systemising’ dimension, Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 6
which is defined as ‘the drive to analyze or construct ‘Motivation’ is here being considered to be systems’ (BaronCohen, 2009, p.71). With motivation to learn something, or the desire to questionnaires, the measure of the peculiarity of gather knowledge (Artelt, 2005). Motivation can be both dimensions – called EQ and SQ – can be seen as ‘time on task’ (Artelt, 2005, p.233) spent determined (Billington et al, 2007). focusing on the subject. If somebody is motivated to learn something, s/he will probably spend more People with a high SQvalue, called ’systemisers’, time on it. are generally more engaged in science than people who are stronger in empathising (Zeyer et al, 2013, There are several constructs concerning p.1047). In BaronCohen’s studies, it seems that the motivation. Following Glynn & Koballa (2006), two dimensions are independent from each other. these are, for instance, intrinsic/extrinsic BaronCohen and his colleagues calculated the motivation, goal orientation, selfconfidence, self difference between the EQ and SQvalue and determination and anxiety (Glynn & Koballa, 2006). statistically identified five socalled brain types: Thus, the challenge is to observe different aspects Extreme Empathisers; Empathisers; Balanced; of motivation, knowing that ‘motivation cannot be Systemisers; and Extreme Systemisers. Further observed directly’ (Barth, 2010). Different types of studies showed that the two dimensions do not instruments measure the amount of motivation, depend on each other and the concept of brain such as the Leuven’s scale of types was sometimes misleading, because a person involvement/engagement (Laevers, 2007). Within can have a balanced brain type, with either two this measure, Laevers specified different signs of similarly high EQ and SQvalues or two similar motivation: bodily posture, attentiveness, minor values (SvedholmHäkkinen & Lindeman, endurance, accuracy, responsiveness and 2015, p.366). contentment. If we assume that someone is motivated when s/he follows attentively in a Zeyer et al (2013) showed that only the SQvalue situation, we can observe the different focus of has an impact on the motivation to do science. So attention that the children choose in the scientific far, the relation between the SQvalue and learning situations, and their duration. motivation for science has not been tested with young children, only with high school students. However, the EQ and SQvalues can be measured Early years science in German kindergartens for 411 yearold children using a combined 55item In German kindergartens it is common practice to EQSQ Child Questionnaire, which was validated in use two different approaches to do science. The a large study with over 1500 participants (Auyeung main difference between these two approaches is et al, 2009). In this case, the parents filled out the in the degree of structuring of the didactical and questionnaire for their children. methodic arrangements used. An aspect that both ways have in common is that the learning In this current study, the goal is to find out whether environment often starts with the exploration of there are differences in motivation between a natural phenomenon. systemisers and empathisers when attending scientific learning environments at kindergarten The first applied approach is ‘rather structured’, (Skorsetz & Welzel, 2015). Maybe children with because the idea is that the child coconstructs new different brain types need different forms of access knowledge with others: for example, in a to science (Zeyer et al, 2013, p.1047)? structured experiment an instruction is followed by an interpretation and guided by questions and interventions of the teacher (Lück, 2009). In this What is motivation? way, the learning environment is led and structured Before we can find out how motivated preschool by the preschool teacher. The preschool teacher children are when participating in scientific learning and the children are often sitting around a table. environments, we have to first define the term The materials to be used are displayed by the ‘motivation’. A useful definition is: ‘Motivation is an teacher on a dark pad and labelled by both teacher internal condition that elicits, leads and maintains and children. A manual is used, which is followed the children’s behaviour’ (Glynn & Koballa, 2006). using a stepbystep procedure. Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 7
The experimentation phase is followed by an p RQ1: To what extent do preschool children show interpretation phase, where the children try empathising or systemising characteristics? to find an explanation for the phenomenon. At first, all tested children in the first year of the For the other approach, the idea is that the project and of both brain types participated in a child makes holistic (nature) experiences together more structured setting. In the following year, with others, in a playful way and in a other tested children (the ‘next generation’), again communicative setting. Hence, s/he has the of both brain types, participated in the more possibility to identify him/herself with somebody exploratory learning environment. So, our research else, or with a situation in a social setting, for question is specified for the two settings: example through a fictional framing story (Schäfer, 2008, 2015). The children and the teacher are often p RQ2: To what extent is the influence of brain type sitting on the floor in a circle and the materials are or of the EQ and SQvalue reflected in displayed. A framing story is ‘told’ by a puppet, for differences in children‘s actions in a ‘rather example, and the story ends with a problem structured’ (RQ 2.1), or a ‘rather open’ (RQ 2.2), encountered by the puppet, which has to be solved learning environment based on the behaviour by the children. After the story, the children have chosen for measurement and its duration? time to explore the materials or the phenomenon and solve the problem in their own way, in order to ‘help’ the puppet. Method In order to answer the research questions, our Research questions study was organised in three steps: The main goal of this study is to find out how pre school children with different EQ/SQvalues act p (1): implementation of the EQSQ Child and react in different didactical and methodical Questionnaire (developed by Auyeung et al, learning environments, on the same scientific 2009); topic. In other words, we are observing children p (2): implementation of the more structured in the two different learning environments in order learning setting, and of the rather exploratory to investigate potential different behaviour. type of learning environment; and p (3): analysis of correlations between the brain Our hypotheses in this context are: type of the children and their actions in the different learning settings. p H1: Systemisers could be more motivated to do science in more structured learning (1): At first we had to translate and validate the environments because of their higher SQvalue, EQSQ Child Questionnaire (Auyeung et al, 2009) which leads them to search for systems. in order to determine the EQ and SQvalues of p H2: According to Zeyer et al (2013), we assume every child in a communicative validation process that fictional stories and the possible (Lamnek et al, 2010). The questionnaire was given identification with protagonists should to the children’s parents because of the young especially motivate empathisers to do science. age of the children participating in the study. An additional idea is that learning environments The tested children were 56 years old and were in that include time to explore the materials could the last year of kindergarten before entering be motivating for empathisers. primary school. Based on these hypotheses, we developed the (2): In order to measure different actions following research questions in order to find concerning the children’s motivation and to differences between the children in two contrasting investigate if these were independent of the learning environments. First, we have to find out didactical and methodological arrangement of the whether different EQ and SQvalues can be found learning environment, a twostep procedure was among preschool children: followed, where children participated in one of the Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 8
two contrasting scientific learning environments. This result is in accordance with the literature Both learning environments were based on the (Auyeung et al, 2009). Thus, we can conclude that same scientific phenomenon: ‘absorbency the translated questionnaire was valid and reliable. properties of different materials’ (Krahn, 2005). Overall, 112 children were tested by the The learning environments were theorybased and questionnaire during data collection in the spring of evaluated using the Design Based Research 2015 and spring 2016. approach (DBR Collective, 2007). Development, implementation and analysis One of the mixed groups of tested children of the learning environment participated in the ‘rather structured’ approach; the Both learning environments were based on the other group participated in the ‘rather open’ scientific phenomenon of absorbency. We expected approach. The children’s behaviour (n=50) was the children to recognise this phenomenon from observed (videorecorded) carefully. The same situations experienced at home and in kindergarten procedure was performed with the ‘rather open’ and involving the spilling of fluids. approach in the following year. The videotapes formed the basis for the empirical analysis, using For the study, children with brain type/EQ and inductively developed observational categories SQvalue participated in the learning environment focusing on what the children were looking at in groups of four. All activities were videorecorded (Mayring, 2008). using two video cameras filming the sequences from different angles. During the summers of 2015 (3): The third and last step was to calculate and 2016, 99 preschool children, aged 56, from statistically the correlation between the compiled seven different kindergartens in the area of EQ response and SQvalues with the data from the Heidelberg, Germany, were filmed. video analysis, in order to find the expected significant differences between the two groups of children (Bortz & Döring, 2006). The data collection of the ‘rather structured’ learning environment took place in spring/summer 2015 in 15 settings with 52 children. The data Results collection of the ‘rather exploratory’ learning The EQSQ Questionnaire environment comprised of 14 settings with The analysis of the questionnaire was carried out 47 children, which was implemented in with the participation of different researchers from spring/summer 2016. Hence, the total number of different faculties in order to achieve video material added up to about 10 hours. communicative validation (Lamnek et al, 2010). About 17 scientists, who usually meet regularly The two videotapes of each setting were inputted during a seminar, participated in this twostep in the evaluation software programme Videograph procedure. For the pretest, the first version of the (Rimmele, 2012) and synchronised. Inductively, questionnaire in German was trialled with a mother we developed eight observation categories with and her child in that age group. From this, we the focus on the children’s viewing directions. obtained answers to the questions, as well as These included children looking: comments about the clarity of the questions. After another communicative validation process with the p Towards the preschool teacher above research group, based on the mother’s p Towards other children comments, the second and improved version of the p At the experimentation material questionnaire was finalised. The pilot study p Towards the observer/into the camera followed, with the questionnaire administered to 25 parents of preschool children. The internal p Around consistency of the results was tested statistically. p At material not relevant to the immediate Cronbach’s alpha coefficients were calculated and situation showed acceptable coefficients for empathy items p Indistinguishable (α=0.81), as well as for systemising items (α=0.61). p At anything else Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 9
Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 1 Difference − .69** – .38** − .03 – .16 .16 −.01 –.10 −.18 –.14 2 EQ −.32** .12 −.22 −.11 –.09 –.06 −.09 −.08 3 SQ −.21 .04 .00 −.07 −.02 –.31* –.28* Notes: Difference = Difference EQ/SQ = Brain Type, EQ = relative EQvalue (2); SQ = relative SQvalue (3), Teacher = View towards Preschool Teacher (4), Children = View towards other Children (5), Exp.mat. = View towards the Experimentation Material (6), Cam. = View toward the Observer/into the Camera (7), Around = View around (8), Mat. n. r. = View t. Material that is not relevant right now (9), Distraction (10) * p < .05, ** p < .01. (onetailed) Table 1: Correlations (‘rather structured’ learning environment). Next, we converged categories 4, 5 and 6 into a followed with the ‘rather exploratory’ learning new category, ‘Distraction/ Attentiveness’. The environment. In contrast to the first setting, the 6th category involved material that the children coding of the viewing directions of the children stored in their pockets, or experimentation started just after the end of the presentation of material that had been used before but was no the framing story. longer relevant. The 8th category was not used. A manual was produced. Correlations The videotapes of both learning environments In order to answer the second research question, were then analysed in detail according to the the EQ and SQvalues of each child were manual. All videos were analysed by two coders. correlated using the videoanalysed data. Table 1 In the ‘rather structured’ learning environment, shows the results of the correlations of the data the children’s viewing directions were gathered for from the ‘rather structured’ learning environment the whole setting. With the software programme and the children’s EQ and SQvalue. Two Videograph, the duration was measured as a significant values were identified in this onetailed percentage independent from the duration of the Spearman correlation: r=–.31* (negative setting. Different codes were identified and correlation between SQ and ‘View to material that discussed in the communicative validation process is not relevant right now’, row 3, column 9) and (Lamnek et al, 2010). The same procedure was r=–.28* (negative correlation between SQ and Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1 Difference −.68** .56** −.02 .04 .02 −.03 .01 .34** –.14 −.02 2 EQ .20 –.05 .17 .11 –.02 –.14 .28* −.08 –.14 3 SQ .04 .20 .11 −.01 −.12 –.23 .13 −.16 Notes: Difference = Difference EQ/SQ = Brain Type, EQ = relative EQvalue (2); SQ = relative SQvalue (3), Teacher = View towards Preschool Teacher (4), Children = View towards other Children (5), Exp.mat. = View towards the Experimentation Material (6), Cam. = View toward the Observer/into the Camera (7), Around = View around (8), Mat. n. r. = View t. Material that is not relevant right now (9), Puppet = View towards Handpuppet (10), Distraction (11) Table 2: Correlations (‘rather open’ learning environment). Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 10
‘Distraction’, row 3, column 10). This means that Discussion and conclusions children with a higher SQvalue tend to be more The first conclusion that we drew from the results focused on the scientific related aspects. was that children with a high SQvalue seem to be motivated in both learning environments. In Table 2, the results of the correlation in the ‘rather So, children with a high SQvalue seem to be exploratory’ learning environment with the motivated to do science independent of the children’s EQ and SQvalues are displayed. Our learning environment and the pedagogical study has relevant correlation with a significant approach used. This result matches our first value (r=.28*, row 2, column 9) between the EQ hypothesis with respect to children with a high value and the ‘view towards material that is not SQvalue being motivated to do science in relevant right now’. structured learning environments. The results go beyond this hypothesis, because the children This means that children with a higher EQvalue always seem to be motivated to do science tend to focus on nonrelevant aspects, so they seem whatever the learning environment. to be more distracted than children with a higher However, the second hypothesis can neither be SQvalue. Another significant value is r=–.34** confirmed nor refuted, because we found no hint (variable difference, row 1, column 9) between the that children with a high EQvalue seem to be children’s brain type and the ‘view towards material motivated to do science in the different learning that is not relevant right now’. This result could be environments. Maybe these children prefer to interpreted as children with a higher SQvalue (in focus on the nonrelevant material to contribute the difference accumulated) focusing less on to the learning environment. distracting material. This could also be interpreted as meaning that, again, children with a high SQ Therefore, the significant correlations lead to value are more motivated to do science. the following hypotheses, which need to be investigated further: Limitations p Children with high SQvalues tend to be Looking critically at the data, we have to take into motivated to do science independent from account specific limitations. Firstly, the parents the learning environment; and filled in the EQ/SQ Child Questionnaire and evaluated their own children. Some of their p The ‘rather open’ learning environment answers could be socially desirable. motivates children with high EQvalues due to the possibility of choosing additional Secondly, characteristics (perhaps relevant) other material for their activities. than the EQ and SQvalues were not collected through the questionnaire (e.g. socioeconomical Further analyses of the quantity and choice of background, intelligence, previous knowledge), objects touched and labelled could be an and no longitudinal data of the children were interesting additional focus. available. Thirdly, the influence of the use of small groups when the children participated in the learning environments could not be investigated. Acknowledgement This study is financially supported by the Klaus Additionally, only some selected aspects, such Tschira Foundation and the Forscherstation, the as the focus of the children’s views, which were Klaus Tschira Competence Center for Early Years considered as measures for motivation based on Science Education, which aims to inspire their duration, were observed in this study. enthusiasm for the natural sciences in children of a very young age, by training kindergarten Finally, the comparability of the groups in the and primary school teachers. The Competence sample of 2015 and 2016 might not be entirely Center is an affiliation of the Klaus Tschira accurate given the random composition of pre Foundation and is associated with the Heidelberg school children. University of Education. Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 11
References Mayring, P. (2008) Qualitative Inhaltsanalyse: Artelt, C. (2005) ‘CrossCultural Approaches to Grundlagen und Techniken (10th Edition). Measuring Motivation’, Educational Assessment, Weinheim: Beltz 10, (3), 231–255. Retrieved from: Patrick, H. & Mantzicopoulos, P. (2015) ‘Young https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326977ea1003_5 Children’s Motivation for Learning Science’. In: Auyeung, B., Wheelwright, S., Allison, C., Atkinson, Research in Early Childhood Science Education, M., Samarawickrema, N. & BaronCohen, S. Trundle, K.C. & Saçkes, M. (Eds.), 7–34. (2009) ‘The Children’s Empathy Quotient and Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands Systemizing Quotient: Sex Differences in Typical https://doi.org/10.1007/9789401795050_2 Development and in Autism Spectrum Schäfer, G.E. (2011) Bildungsprozesse im Conditions’, Journal of Autism and Developmental Kindesalter. Selbstbildung, Erfahrung und Disorder, 39, (11) Lernen in der frühen Kindheit. Weinheim und BaronCohen, S. (2009) ‘Autism: The Empathizing München: Juventa Systemizing (ES) Theory’, Annals of the New Skorsetz, N. & WelzelBreuer, M. (2016) York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 68–80 ’Systemizing and Empathizing. Research on Billington, J., BaronCohen, S. & Wheelwright, S. Early Years Science Education and Brain Types’. (2007) ‘Cognitive style predicts entry into In: Systemizing and Empathizing. Electronic physical sciences and humanities: Questionnaire Proceedings of the ESERA 2015 Conference and performance tests of empathy and Science Education Research: Engaging Learners systemizing’, Learning and Individual Differences, for a Sustainable Future, Lavonen, J., Juuti, K., 17, (3), 260–268 Lampiselkä, J., Uitto, A. & Hahl, K. (Hrsg.), Bortz, J. & Döring, N. (2006) Forschungsmethoden 15715, 2599–2607. Retrieved from: und Evaluation. Für Human und www.esera.org/media/eBook%202015/eBook_P Sozialwissenschaftler. Heidelberg: Springer art_15_links.pdf MedizinVerlag Zeyer, A., ÇetinDindar, A., Nurulazam Md Zain, A., Collective, T.D.B.R. (2003) ’DesignBased Juriševič, M., Devetak, I. & Odermatt, F. (2013) Research: An emerging paradigm for ‘Systemizing: A crosscultural constant for educational inquiry’, Educational Researcher, 32, motivation to learn science’, Journal of Research (1), 5–8 in Science Teaching, 50, (9), 1047–1067 Glynn, S.M. & Koballa, T.R. Jr. (2006) ‘Motivation to Learn in College Science’. In: Handbook of College Science Teaching, Mintzes J.J. & Leonhard, W.H. (Eds.). Arlington, VA: National Nina Skorsetz and Manuela WelzelBreuer, Science Teachers Association Press Goethe University Frankfurt/Main and University Krahn, S. (2005) Untersuchungen zum intuitiven of Education, Heidelberg, Germany. naturwissenschaftlichen Wissen von Kindern im Emails: skorsetz@em.unifrankfurt.de and Alter zwischen zwei und sieben Jahren. welzel@phheidelberg. Universität Bielefeld. Retrieved from https://pub.unibielefeld.de/publication/2303984 Laevers, F. (2007) Die Leuvener Engagiertheitsskala. LESK (2nd Edition). Erkelenz: Klara Schlörner Lück, G. (2003) Handbuch der naturwissenschaftlichen Bildung. Theorie und Praxis für die Arbeit in Kindertageseinrichtungen. Freiburg im Breisgau: Herder Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 12
Pre-school children’s collaborative science learning scaffolded by tablets – A teacher’s view l Marie Fridberg l Susanne Thulin l Andreas Redfors (Nihlfors, 2008; Gitomer & Zisk, 2015). Research This paper has also appeared in: Finlayson, O., also points to some key factors where teachers’ McLoughlin, E., Erduran, S. & Childs, P. (Eds.) knowledge of science is one central issue for the (2018) Electronic Proceedings of the ESERA 2017 learning (SirajBlatchford et al, 2002; Yoshikawa, Conference. Research, Practice and 2013). Furthermore, Fleer (2009) expresses the link Collaboration in Science Education. Dublin, between early childhood teachers’ limited science Ireland: Dublin City University. knowledge and teachers’ confidence and ISBN 9781873769843. competence to teach science. However she, Reproduced here with permission from ESERA. together with other researchers, also point to preschool teachers’ pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) and attitude towards science Abstract as having an impact on children’s learning (Fleer, The potential of tablets to support communication 2009; Thulin, 2011; SpectorLevy et al, 2013). during collaborative inquirybased science learning in preschool has previously been reported (Fridberg, In this study, we make use of timelapse Thulin & Redfors, 2017). Children communicate in a photography and Slowmations. Timelapse more advanced manner about the phenomenon and photography is a technique that shows a slowly they focus more readily on problemsolving when changing event in accelerated speed, which is active in experimentation or Slowmation production. accomplished by photographing the event at Here, we shift focus to the preschool teacher’s certain intervals and, when played at normal perspective on science and the work model with speed, the event seems much faster. A Slowmation timelapse and Slowmation. The preschool teacher’s on the other hand is a stopmotion animation talk about the teaching was analysed from three played in slow motion to explain a science concept perspectives: the relationships between teacher (Hoban, 2007). The work model implemented with science, teacherchildren, and childrenscience. children aged 3 to 6 (Fridberg, Thulin & Redfors, Possibilities and challenges expressed by the pre 2017) constitutes four different learning contexts: school teacher in relation to the three perspectives i.e. handson experiments, timelapse photography, were identified and, interestingly, the analysis shows stimulated recall, followed by Slowmation creation, most possibilities in the childrenscience relationship. where the children represent explanatory models In contrast, most challenges are found for the in different materials, and which is versatile and teacherscience relationship, in terms of lack of opens up possibilities for the children to generate, knowledge. We argue for the need to further discuss represent and discuss explanatory models. pre and inservice preschool teachers’ experiences of science and science education. From a theoretical framework primarily based on phenomenography and variation theory Keywords: Preschool, science, tablets (Marton & Booth, 1997), focusing on developmental pedagogy (Pramling Samuelsson & Asplund Carlsson, 2008), this work aims to Introduction analyse variations of, in retrospect, expressed One identified important factor for children’s experiences by the teacher, for the four different learning, whether in preschool or in the education contexts of science learning during a semi system as a whole, is teachers’ content knowledge structured interview. Main Article JES14 Summer 2018 page 13
The research question guiding the study is: Results The challenges and possibilities identified in the p What differences in referential meaning can be analysis of the teacher’s statements involving ascribed to the teacher’s statements about the science and the work model will below be science teaching work model, especially summarised as belonging to the themes: concerning possibilities and challenges? knowledge, number of children, children’s previous experiences, time constraints, and interactions. Method Challenges described by the teacher include his A semistructured interview with openended experienced lack of knowledge in science, even questions was performed with the teacher one year though he also stated that the project increased his after the finalisation of the project. The teacher knowledge. Another challenge expressed is answered spontaneously, with followup questions connected to numbers, when too many children from the researcher. In addition, we used take part in the activities. This was manifested in stimulated recall where the teacher watched the teacher expressing the need to take a more selected parts of the videorecorded activities with controlling role in the situation, compared to when the children, and was asked to reflect on his role. there are fewer children involved. Fewer children The activities were chosen to represent four allow for him to stand back and the discussions enacted learning contexts analysed previously among the children were more fruitful and less (Fridberg et al, 2017). The teacher’s statements stressful. However, in addition, the teacher were analysed and categorised with specific stressed that the number of children is only one focuses, depicted in Figure 1. factor influencing the outcome. The results achieved during the production of Slowmations, Through a phenomenographic analysis, we could etc. also depend on the individual children involved identify variations of experienced possibilities and and their previous experiences and ability to co challenges with parts of the science teaching work operate. Yet another experienced limiting factor is model, as described by the teacher. time and the pressure the teacher felt to reach conclusions and ‘get somewhere’ during the timeframe of the activity. This resulted in the Teacher teacher sometimes seeing himself as someone who provided the answers too quickly, or feeling that he did not give the children space to think through the activity. He described how the lived/felt time pressure sometimes negatively influenced the A discussions between him and the children. B The teacher reported that, during the project, he came to expand his view of natural science from one restricted to biology to then include also chemistry and physics, something that can be thought of as opening up opportunities. This result Science teaching C Children also concurs with the results of Thulin and Redfors work model (2017), who found a large portion of students having changed their views accordingly from pre Figure 1: The triangle of analysis depicts the three to posttest related to a science course in pre relations A, B, C focused in the analysis of the school teacher education. teacher’s statements, related to A) the object of learning in terms of the science teaching work The possibilities described lie in the work model model, B) the children, and C) the children’s itself. It takes into consideration the children’s relation to science or the work model. interest in tablets and captures many of the areas Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 14
in the preschool curriculum, such as natural for students when they construct their own science, mathematics and language, but also representations of scientific processes and values and interaction. The work model, with concepts. These gains include the development of discussions, handson experiments and students’ cognitive and reasoning strategies, the timelapse/Slowmation production, where the promotion of contexts that engender meaningful children represent the phenomenon in different communication of science understandings, and of materials, is versatile and opens up the learning of activities that are highly engaging for students. Our the science phenomenon from several results were in agreement and revealed the perspectives. This, according to the teacher, is timelapse movies as beneficial for the children’s consistent with the preschool teacher’s mission to modelbased reasoning about the science content include all children based on their individual needs (evaporation), and the Slowmation movie and preconditions. production helped the children to think about explanations for, and representations of, the Most possibilities described by the teacher are science content. Another interesting aspect of found in the relationship between the children and young children and their interaction with digital the object of learning (side C in the triangle, Figure technology is described by Kjällander and Moinian 1). He stated that the teachers and their attitudes (2014). They studied preschool children’s playful to the work model or science are the limiting use of applications on tablets and described how factor, not the children. These results can be their interactions resisted the preexisting didactic compared to Thulin (2011), who stated that the design of the application. Instead, the children in children were interested in the content and asked their study transformed the application setting and contentrelated questions. The children have no objects into something that made sense and had difficulties working with experiments, movie meaning for them. The children designed their own production, etc. The results depend on the attitude process by making sense of affordances provided of the teacher, who needs to be positive in order to by the digital resource in relation to their own have a fruitful outcome. Interestingly, and to the interest and previous experience. This finding contrary, most challenges are described in the reflects children’s agency and opens up the way relationship between the teacher himself and the for thoughts on consumer and production object of learning (side A in Figure 1), in terms of processes in the digital arena (Kjällander & experienced lack of knowledge. Moinian, 2014). We consider our proposed work model with timelapse and Slowmation production to fit well into an agentic view of childhood Discussion (Fridberg et al, 2017). In the present study, we An increasing number of studies (Ainsworth, 1999; expand our previous work, which focused on the Prain & Waldrip, 2006) suggest that students’ children, to include the teacher’s views and learning is enhanced when they create digital experience of science and the jointly developed artefacts, such as representations of science work model. concepts. Through this creation of an explanatory model, skills such as creativity, problemsolving, In order to help children to learn about something, communication and collaboration can be it is important that the teacher considers children’s developed (Nielsen & Hoban, 2015). We have previous experiences, as well as the object of previously studied and proposed a work model for learning (Pramling Samuelsson & Asplund Carlsson, children’s collaborative science learning through 2008; Thulin, 2011; Gustavsson, Jonsson, Ljung the use of computer tablets. This teaching model Djärf & Thulin, 2016). During the interview, the includes discussions, experiments documented by teacher highlighted the importance of the timelapse photography, stimulated recall for the children’s prior experiences when he stated that teacher and children through watching timelapse ‘It’s about what they have in their luggage too’ as he movies, and a subsequent production of a described the lived curriculum. It is not the age of Slowmation movie, where the children represented the children, but their prior experiences, which set their explanatory model in, for instance, playdough the limits according to the teacher and this concurs or LEGO® (Fridberg et al, 2017). Prain and Tytler with the variation theory as described by Marton (2013) argue that there are particular learning gains (2015). Important for a teacher to consider is what Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 15
the learners have not yet learned to discern about Interestingly, and striking from the analysis, most a certain object of learning, since these ‘lacking challenges are expressed in the teacherlearning pieces’ are critical aspects for further learning. object relationship (A) and in terms of the experienced lack of knowledge, while most In a recent national report, Skolinspektionen (2016) possibilities are described in the childrenlearning describes how the science subject in Swedish pre object relationship (C). Different children show schools often is acted out in somewhat random interest in different parts of the versatile work experiment activities, without guidance towards a model, and the teacher viewed them as capable. specific object of learning. The teacher in the Instead, he pointed out that it is the knowledge and present study pointed out the benefits of the work attitude of the preschool teacher towards science model, including several subjects and aspects of and the work model that will determine possible science such as, for instance, language, technology opportunities and challenges. In this case study, the and mathematics. teacher was stimulated by the work model, but was less well prepared for the science content. During the work process, the teacher also saw Research on primary science teachers of young other important parts of the curriculum being children shows that teachers have insufficient expressed, such as value systems and the children’s knowledge of the subject and pedagogical content ability to interact with each other. He suggested knowledge (Appleton, 2008; Fleer, 2009; Thulin, the work model to be used prior to a parent 2011; SpectorLevy et al, 2013). Other studies also teacher conference, since it would enable him as a point to early childhood teachers’ lack of science teacher to learn things about the children and their subject matter knowledge (Kallery & Psillos, 2001; capabilities that he didn’t know about. The teacher Garbett, 2003) but, to date, only a few studies have talked about children whom he knew to be a bit shy focused on practising inservice preschool and withdrawn surprising him by being active and teachers (Andersson & Gullberg, 2014). This relates expressing their thoughts during the timelapse and to justification for science in preschool and the Slowmation production. He connected this to the ‘being’ and ‘becoming’ perspectives, where ‘being’ preconceptions and takenforgranted refers to viewing children as actors in their own assumptions that adults and teachers often have lives and letting them meet the science primarily about children’s capabilities. for their own sake, while the ‘becoming’ refers to future uses of an early grounded knowledge. Structural factors challenging the work with the Thulin & Redfors (2017) do not polarise the two model and science included the number of children perspectives, but show that student preschool participating in the activity: the more children, the teachers revealed a slight predominance for the more stressful a situation for the teacher to handle. society – ‘becoming’ – perspective. However, from However, he also pointed to the influence of which the results presented here, we can say that both children one, as a teacher, groups together in the teacher and children benefited from and activities, something that may be viewed as a experienced a ‘being’ perspective concerning their relational factor that either limits or enables the modelbased reasoning. learning. If the children cooperate easily with each other, the group can be larger. Another In this study, different dilemmas that the teacher structural factor challenging his work included an encountered when working with science and the experienced lack of time during the activities. work model could be identified. In the teacher’s This, according to the teacher, influenced his own words, ‘It’s a balancing act all the time, it’s discussions with the children in a negative way, really tricky’. The teacher has to consider and and prompted him to provide them with the handle several factors at the same time, such as correct answer to his questions, or to move on too time management and children’s interactions, fast without giving the children time to think as when teaching a subject manifested in the much as he would have liked them to. However, the curriculum about which he is not really comfortable main structural factor expressed by the teacher as with his knowledge. He talked about the need for limiting for his work is his experienced lack of support, from both the children’s and the teacher’s knowledge in science. perspective. The children are dependent on him Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 16
and his attitude towards the subject, and he in turn unexpected things that happen in the moment; needs support in the form of someone with whom asking challenging questions to further to discuss ideas, thoughts and questions. Or, in investigations; and ‘situated presence’, that is, other words, the need for someone who supports remaining in the situation and listening to the his work through discussions about the intended children. By making use of these skills, preschool and enacted object of learning. In Thulin & Redfors teachers may shift focus from their experienced (2017), the majority of student preschool teachers insufficient subject matter knowledge to instead were positive about science and this result is reinforce their pedagogical content knowledge contrary to the general impression in western (Andersson & Gullberg, 2014). countries that young people share negative views of science. Like the student preschool teachers in At the same time, we argue that a polarisation the Thulin & Redfors survey, the preschool teacher between science and science education is not in the present study expressed a positive attitude beneficial. In intertwining support of science content to science. Instead, it was a feeling of not knowing with implementation of work models such as the enough about different science matters that the one presented here, both knowledge of explanatory teacher in this study saw as the challenge in his models in science and competence in handling work. This is further reflected in what we, as activities with children can be seen to improve. teachers in the preschool teacher programme, encounter in our work with students and the science subjects. Our experience is that the Conclusion and implications students struggle with modelbased reasoning, The timelapse/Slowmation work model shows while the attitude towards the subject is one of promise for future developmental work concerning positivity and curiosity among most students. both children and teachers. We agree with Eshach (2006) that support for teachers’ work with science This raises an important question. When different content in preschool is needed, and the work content in the Swedish preschool curriculum is model described by us could be viewed as one such discussed, it is often from the children’s support structure, through its framing and perspectives, and about how the content can be meaningmaking of the science content. The work presented to fit the children’s interests and model with timelapse and Slowmation production previous experiences. Eshach (2006) reasons that concurs with teaching from a child perspective and the teacher perspective is equally important to an agentic role of active children in collaboration consider when working with science in early years: (Fridberg et al, 2017). In the present study, where previous work has been expanded to include the ‘I thus argue that educators should seek after such teacher’s views and experience, we have found science activities that not only fit the children’s needs several distinct and important implications for pre but also the teachers’ abilities, motivations and and inservice preschool teacher education. needs. (…) But to succeed in using such an approach, a kindergarten teacher must receive sufficient Results from this research on uses of our work scientific support. (…) Thus, I argue that K2 science model for science teaching have given a list of education should be teachercentered as well as implications – four ‘pointstomake’ for teacher studentcentered, as opposed to the traditional education and preschool practice. studentcentered approach’ (Eshach, 2006). Firstly, teaching with and about the work model entails and highlights the importance and Here, and in earlier work, we have seen the fruitfulness of having planned teaching activities importance and benefits of intertwining science with formulated intended objects of learning, content and science education research results in which open up several overarching learning goals in working with inservice preschool teachers – a the curriculum, such as communication, co view supported by Andersson and Gullberg (2014), operation and world views, and bring in aspects of who suggest four skills of which preschool other subjects, including language, technology and teachers can make use when teaching science: mathematics. Secondly, it combines digital children’s previous experiences; capturing technology and science learning objects. Thirdly, Main Article JES15 Summer 2018 page 17
You can also read