The Effect of Rumination and Reappraisal on Social Anxiety Symptoms During Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Social Anxiety Disorder

Page created by Arnold Moore
 
CONTINUE READING
The Effect of Rumination and Reappraisal on Social Anxiety
Symptoms During Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Social Anxiety
Disorder
Faith A. Brozovich,1 Philippe Goldin,2 Ihno Lee,1 Hooria Jazaieri,3
Richard G. Heimberg,4 and James J. Gross1
1
  Stanford University
2
  University of California, Davis
3
  University of California, Berkeley
4
  Temple University

Objective: There is growing interest in the role of transdiagnostic processes in the onset, mainte-
nance, and treatment of mental disorders (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011). Two such transdiagnos-
tic processes–rumination and reappraisal–are the focus of the present study. The main objective was
to examine the roles of rumination (thought to be harmful) and reappraisal (thought to be helpful) in
adults with social anxiety disorder (SAD). Method:         We conducted a randomized controlled trial of
cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) with 75 adults with SAD and examined pre- to post-CBT changes as
well as weekly fluctuations in rumination, reappraisal, and social anxiety symptoms. Results:           So-
cially anxious individuals’ baseline rumination (brooding) scores predicted weekly levels of social anxiety,
rumination, and reappraisal, whereas baseline reappraisal scores did not. Greater weekly rumination
was associated with greater weekly social anxiety, but reappraisal was not related to social anxiety.
Conclusion: These findings suggest that rumination may have a more significant role than reap-
praisal in understanding fluctuations in social anxiety during CBT for SAD.   C 2014 Wiley Periodicals,

Inc. J. Clin. Psychol. 71:208–218, 2015.

Keywords: CBT; social anxiety; emotion regulation; cognitive reappraisal; rumination; brooding

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterized by an intense fear of negative evaluation in social
situations (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). SAD can be extremely debilitating,
causing considerable impairment in education, employment, and social functioning (Aderka
et al., 2012). Thus, the effect of SAD is substantial.
    Cognitive-behavioral models of SAD highlight aberrant emotional responses, cognitions, and
behaviors that are unique to social anxiety (Clark & Wells, 1995; Rapee & Heimberg, 1997). Ac-
cording to these models, problems with emotional reactivity and regulation in SAD are a result
of distorted beliefs and interpretations, as well as safety behaviors and other avoidance behaviors
employed to cope with heightened levels of anxiety. These models have focused on specificity,
illustrating the ways in which social anxiety is distinct from other psychological disorders.
However, as clinical science progresses, there is growing interest in the role of transdiagnos-
tic processes in the onset, maintenance, and treatment of mental disorders (Nolen-Hoeksema
& Watkins, 2011). Many potential mechanisms exist; however, in the present study, we chose to
focus on two cognitive emotion regulation phenomena: the maladaptive process of rumination,
specifically brooding, and the adaptive process of reappraisal. We were particularly interested
in these two mechanisms because both are cognitive and malleable in nature. We also were
interested in examining the effects of cognitive-behavioral therapy (CBT) on these mechanisms.

Please address correspondence to: Faith A. Brozovich, Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall, Bldg. 420,
Stanford, CA 94205-2130. E-mail: faithb@stanford.edu

JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 71(3), 208–218 (2015)                                    
                                                                                              C 2014 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.

Published online in Wiley Online Library (wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jclp).   DOI: 10.1002/jclp.22132
Rumination, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety                         209

Rumination as a Maladaptive Process
The largest body of empirical work examining the maladaptive process of rumination has em-
ployed Nolen-Hoeksema’s (1991) definition of rumination: a cognitive process in which individ-
uals unproductively perseverate on their mood and related symptoms, causes, and consequences.
Early studies identified rumination as an important component of major depressive disorder
(MDD; Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Since that time, rumination has been linked to increases in
symptoms of several other disorders, including anxiety, alcohol, substance use, and eating dis-
orders (Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, & Schweizer, 2010).
   Treynor, Gonzalez, and Nolen-Hoeksema (2003) delineated two subtypes of rumination,
brooding–“a passive comparison of one’s current situation with some unachieved standard”–
and reflective pondering–“purposefully turning inward to engage in cognitive problem-solving”
(p. 256). Brooding has been identified as a particularly insidious form of rumination in MDD
(Pearson, Watkins, Kuyken, & Mullan, 2010). Brooding also has been strongly associated with
anxiety symptoms in a clinical setting (Watkins, 2009). Interestingly, although studies have shown
a link between rumination and anxiety, research on rumination and particularly brooding in
SAD, a disorder characterized by increased levels of negative ruminative thinking, is limited.
   Cognitive-behavioral models of SAD posit that after a social event, individuals with SAD
engage in a “post mortem” or review of the social situation (Clark & Wells, 1995; Heimberg,
Brozovich, & Rapee, 2010). During these episodes of post-event processing (also known as
post-event rumination), individuals with SAD tend to focus on negative information about
themselves and others’ opinions of them during the social situation and compare this to an
unrealistically high standard (for a review, see Brozovich & Heimberg, 2008). Research on
socially anxious individuals who engage in rumination suggests that they experience increases in
anxiety and negative affect over time (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2011) and have distorted memories
of the actual event (Morgan & Banjeree, 2008). Studies that have experimentally manipulated
rumination have also shown that rumination maintains negative beliefs in persons with social
anxiety (Vassilopoulos & Watkins, 2009). A few treatment studies have demonstrated that CBT
reduces rumination (e.g., Price & Anderson, 2011).
   Although rumination has been identified as a maladaptive factor in MDD (Pearson et al.,
2010), only two studies have investigated maladaptive rumination (brooding) in socially anxious
individuals. Thus far, brooding has been shown to be elevated in socially anxious individuals
compared to healthy individuals, but similar to individuals with MDD (Joormann, Dkane,
& Gotlib, 2006) and to mediate the relationship between social anxiety and trait anger (Trew
& Alden, 2009). These studies provide preliminary support for the association between brooding
and social anxiety. Further research is needed to examine the effect of brooding on fluctuations
in social anxiety symptoms and whether brooding affects SAD treatment outcome.

Reappraisal as an Adaptive Process
Alternatively, cognitive reappraisal is considered an adaptive emotion regulation process. Reap-
praisal refers to reinterpreting a potentially evocative stimulus in a way that alters an emotional
response (Gross, 2014). It is generally considered adaptive because it reduces emotional reactiv-
ity, decreases physiological arousal, and improves well-being (Gross, 2014). It is thus no accident
that reappraisal is a fundamental component of cognitive restructuring, a prominent therapeutic
technique in CBT. It is implemented to reduce anxiety or distress when individuals are faced
with negative automatic thoughts or challenging situations. The duration of a full course of
individual CBT provides an excellent time frame in which to study changes in the development
of an adaptive emotion regulation strategy (i.e., reappraisal) over time.
   Emerging evidence suggests that individuals with SAD are less able to implement reap-
praisal and have lower self-efficacy regarding actually applying reappraisal (Goldin, Manber-
Ball, Werner, Heimberg, & Gross, 2009; Werner, Goldin, Ball, Heimberg, & Gross, 2011). As
might be expected given the link between cognitive restructuring and reappraisal, CBT in-
creases reappraisal use (Moscovitch et al., 2012). Moreover, Goldin and colleagues (2012) found
that reappraisal self-efficacy mediated the effect of CBT on social anxiety symptoms and that
210                         Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2015

increases in reappraisal self-efficacy predicted decreases in social anxiety symptoms at 1-year
post-CBT.
   Unfortunately, few studies have directly compared generally adaptive processes, such as reap-
praisal, with generally maladaptive processes, such as rumination. What we know from studies
of anxiety and depression suggests that these forms of psychopathology are more highly asso-
ciated with increased maladaptive processes (rumination and expressive suppression) and more
weakly associated with adaptive processes (reappraisal and problem solving; Aldao et al., 2010).
D’Avanzato, Joormann, Siemer, and Gotlib (2013) found greater use of maladaptive strategies
(rumination and expressive suppression) and lesser use of adaptive strategies (reappraisal) in
individuals with SAD and MDD. Specifically in individuals with SAD, rumination predicted
state anxiety levels. Clearly, cross-sectional designs have provided valuable insight, highlighting
rumination as a maladaptive process linked to social anxiety; however, the broader issue of how
maladaptive and adaptive processes predict symptoms and unfold with respect to changes in
symptom patterns in social anxiety disorder has yet to be examined prospectively.

The Present Study
The goal of the present study was to contrast a generally maladaptive cognitive form of emo-
tion regulation, rumination, with a generally adaptive one, reappraisal. More specifically, we
sought to examine the associations with social anxiety symptoms as rumination and reappraisal
fluctuated during CBT for SAD. We selected a sample of adults with generalized SAD and con-
ducted a randomized controlled trial (RCT) of individual CBT for SAD. We measured baseline
rumination (brooding) and reappraisal as well as assessed weekly fluctuations in rumination,
reappraisal, and social anxiety symptoms.
   We hypothesized that (a) greater baseline rumination (brooding) would predict greater weekly
social anxiety and rumination and lesser weekly reappraisal during CBT, whereas greater base-
line reappraisal would predict lesser weekly social anxiety and rumination and greater weekly
reappraisal. Directly comparing the two predictors, we hypothesized that brooding would be a
more robust predictor of fluctuations in social anxiety than reappraisal. We also expected that
(b) greater weekly rumination during CBT would be related to greater weekly social anxiety and
that greater use of reappraisal would be related to lower levels of weekly social anxiety.

                                            Method
Participants
To assess eligibility for an RCT of CBT for SAD, 436 individuals were screened by
telephone, and of these, 110 were administered a clinical diagnostic interview (see Fig-
ure 1). As part of a larger project focused on the neural bases of CBT for SAD, the
75 socially anxious individuals who met Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental Dis-
orders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; APA, 1994) criteria for a principal diagnosis of generalized
SAD were randomly assigned to either immediate CBT (n = 38) or a waitlist (WL) group
(n = 37). Individuals assigned to WL were subsequently offered CBT. We offered individuals
with SAD free treatment in exchange for their participation. See Table 1 for information on
demographic characteristics. All participants provided informed consent in accordance with the
Institutional Review Board at Stanford University.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
We required that all participants be 21–55 years of age, speak fluent English, be right-handed,
pass a Magnetic Resonance Imaging safety screen, and be free of current pharmacotherapy or
psychotherapy, history of medical disorders (e.g., hypertension, thyroid disorders, diabetes, or
anemia), head trauma, and neurological disorders. Participants also met the DSM-IV criteria
for a principal diagnosis of generalized SAD as assessed by the Anxiety Disorders Interview
Schedule for the DSM-IV-Lifetime version (ADIS-IV-L; DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1994).
Rumination, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety                               211

Table 1
Characteristics of Participants in Immediate Versus Delayed Treatment Groups

Variable                                           I-CBT                 WL                   t or χ
                                                   n = 38              n = 37
Gender (males, n, %)                             19 (50.0%)          20 (54.1%)        χ2 = 0.35, p = .55
Age (yrs, M ± SD)                               33.38 ± 7.60       33.63 ± 10.37       t = −0.12, p = .91
Education (yrs, M ± SD)                         16.68 ± 2.21        16.96 ± 2.51       t = −0.51, p = .61
Ethnicity (n, %)                                                                       χ2 = 4.41, p = .62
  Caucasian                                     22 (57.9%)           21 (56.8%)
  Asian                                          9 (23.7%)            9 (24.3%)
  Hispanic                                        3 (7.9%)            5 (13.5%)
  Other                                           2 (5.3%)            2 (5.4%)
  Multiracial                                     2 (5.3%)              0 (0%)
Baseline brooding (M ± SD)                     12.84 ± 4.29         12.72 ± 3.24        t = 0.14, p = .89
Baseline reappraisal (M ± SD)                  28.42 ± 11.96       28.77 ± 11.22       t = −0.13, p = .90
Baseline weekly rumination (M ± SD)            70.63 ± 25.64       68.18 ± 19.18        t = 0.38, p = .71
Baseline weekly reappraisal (M ± SD)           32.81 ± 24.53       36.82 ± 24.38       t = −0.59, p = .58
Baseline weekly social anxiety (M ± SD)        61.88 ± 20.70       68.18 ± 19.18       t = −1.13, p = .26

Note. I-CBT = individual cognitive-behavioral therapy; WL = waitlist; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.

Figure 1. Consolidated standards of reporting trials diagram for randomized controlled trial of individual
cognitive-behavioral therapy (I-CBT) versus wait-list (WL) control. ITT = intent-to-treat analyses; tx =
treatment.

Generalized SAD was defined as experiencing greater than moderate fear in five or more social
situations as well as receiving a rating of 4 or greater for SAD on the 0–8 clinician severity scale.
We excluded individuals with comorbid psychiatric disorders other than generalized anxiety
disorder, specific phobia, panic disorder, and dysthymia. Of the 75 socially anxious participants,
15 were diagnosed with one or more current comorbid disorders, none of which was judged
primary: three panic disorder, 14 generalized anxiety disorder, five specific phobia, and three
dysthymic disorder.
212                          Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2015

Clinical and Individual Difference Materials
Before entering the study, individuals underwent a semistructured diagnostic interview, the
ADIS-IV-L (DiNardo, Brown, & Barlow, 1994). Two Ph.D. clinical psychologists, both of
whom were trained to conduct the interview by watching training tapes and completing test
cases, administered the ADIS-IV-L in this study. The ADIS-IV-L is a highly reliable interview
for the diagnosis of anxiety disorders and related conditions (Brown, Di Nardo, Lehman,
& Campbell, 2001). Specifically, in a sample of patients with a range of anxiety disorders, it has
shown excellent reliability for a principal diagnosis of SAD (ĸ = .77; Brown, Di Nardo, Lehman,
& Campbell, 2001).
   At baseline and post-CBT, participants completed the Ruminative Responses Scale (Treynor
et al., 2003). The scale comprises two subscales: brooding and reflective pondering. We fo-
cused solely on the brooding subscale given that we were mostly interested in brooding
as a maladaptive form of rumination, and its subscale is least contaminated by depressive
symptoms. The brooding subscale depicts moody rumination or “a passive comparison of
one’s current situation with some unachieved standard” (Treynor et al., 2003, p. 256; e.g.,
Why do I always react this way?). Five items are rated on Likert-type scales ranging from
1 (almost never) to 4 (almost always). The brooding subscale has been shown to have good
reliability in other studies (α = .77; Treynor et al., 2003) as well as in the current study (α = .80).
   At baseline and post-CBT, reappraisal was assessed using an expanded version of the Emotion
Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ; Goldin, Manber-Ball, Werner, Heimberg, & Gross, 2009; Gross
& John, 2003). This scale comprises 52 items, divided between reappraisal and suppression items.
We focused solely on the reappraisal scale. Items measuring reappraisal form two subscales:
frequency of use and self-efficacy. For the purposes of this study, we focused on the frequency
of use of reappraisal since it was the best counterpart for brooding frequency. The reappraisal
frequency scale comprises eight items (e.g., I control my emotions by changing the way I think
about the situation I’m in). Participants rated the items on Likert-type scales ranging from 1
(strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). The original ERQ reappraisal scale has shown good
reliability (α = .79; Gross & John, 2003), as has the expanded version of the ERQ reappraisal
scale in the present study (α = .89).
   During the 16 weeks of CBT, weekly fluctuations in rumination, reappraisal, and social
anxiety were measured using a brief weekly questionnaire designed for this study. Rumination
was assessed with the question: How often have you gone over and over in your mind social
interactions after they have finished? Reappraisal was assessed with the question: How often
did you try to change the way you were thinking about the situation you were in? Participants
rated their responses on Likert-type scales ranging from 0 (not at all; 0%) to 100 (always; 100%).
Weekly social anxiety symptoms were measured with another item: “How intense has your social
anxiety been?” Participants rated this question on a Likert-type scale ranging from 0 (not at
all) to 100 (extremely). Participants completed this assessment–along with additional items to
be reported elsewhere–at baseline, weekly during CBT, monthly during WL, and immediately
post-WL CBT.

CBT for SAD
Participants received individual CBT for SAD based on the therapist manual by Hope, Heim-
berg, and Turk (2006). The individual treatment was completed over the course of 16 weeks in
1-hour weekly sessions, with one 1.5-hour session at the start of in-session exposures. Treatment
comprised five phases: (a) psychoeducation, approximately four sessions; (b) cognitive restruc-
turing training, two to three sessions; (c) in-session and in vivo exposure, eight to nine sessions;
(d) advanced core beliefs work, one to two sessions; and (e) treatment consolidation and termi-
nation, one session. Individuals received a workbook (Hope, Heimberg, Juster, & Turk, 2000)
and were asked to read the appropriate chapters before each therapy session and complete the
relevant homework assignments. All participants completed all 16 sessions and at least seven
in-session exposures. Therapists worked with clients to reschedule sessions due to illness or
vacations.
Rumination, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety                         213

   Four Ph.D. clinical psychologists delivered the treatment. Richard Heimberg, one of the
developers of the treatment protocol, trained all therapists. Therapists needed to achieve com-
petence conducting this CBT protocol with training cases before treating individuals in the
study. All therapy sessions were audio-recorded and then rated for adherence and fidelity us-
ing the Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for Social Anxiety Disorder: Therapist Adherence Scale
(Hope, VanDyke, Heimberg, Turk, & Fresco, 2001). The adherence raters made their responses
on Likert-type scales ranging from 1 (ineffective) to 5 (extremely effective). A one-way random
effects model was conducted with absolute agreement to compute the average intraclass correla-
tion (.81) between raters. Ratings for each of the five components had to be ࣙ 4 to be considered
“in protocol.” The four therapists achieved this standard (overall mean [M] = 4.61, standard
deviation [SD] = 0.24), and a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) showed no differences
among the therapists F(3,33) = 0.11, p = .96.

Procedure
Participants were recruited through referrals, community flyers, and Web listings. After passing
an initial telephone interview and an in-person diagnostic interview, participants were invited
to enroll in the study. After completing baseline assessments, socially anxious individuals were
randomly assigned to CBT or WL using Efron’s biased coin randomization procedure (Efron,
1971), which ensures equal sample sizes throughout the clinical trial. During the 16 weeks of
CBT, participants completed weekly assessments of social anxiety, rumination, and reappraisal
(described in more detail above). After CBT or WL, individuals with SAD were readministered
the same battery of assessments that were completed at baseline.

Statistical Analyses
As part of our preliminary analyses, we conducted between-group t tests on the baseline and
weekly variables to ensure there were no differences between our immediate CBT participants
and post-WL CBT participants. In addition, we conducted repeated measures ANOVAs to
examine pre- to post-CBT changes in rumination (RRS brooding subscale) and reappraisal
(ERQ). For all analyses, we report partial eta-squared (η2 p ) as an index of effect size. Cohen
(1988) identified parameters for η2 p effect sizes as follows: small = .01, medium = .09, and
large = .25.
   We conducted multilevel modeling (MLM) analyses to examine average levels of weekly
social anxiety, rumination, and reappraisal, as a function of person-level variables (baseline
rumination [brooding] and reappraisal) as well as the relationship between weekly social anxiety
and weekly rumination and reappraisal. See Hox (2002) and Snijders and Bosker (1999) for
additional MLM details and specification. We ran two sets of MLM analyses to determine (a)
whether baseline rumination (brooding) or reappraisal predict social anxiety, rumination, and
reappraisal throughout CBT, and (b) whether weekly rumination and reappraisal are associated
with the intensity of weekly social anxiety over the course of CBT. All intercepts and slopes
associated with week-level (vs. person-level) variables were specified as random (i.e., varying
across individuals; represented by u components in Models 1 and 2).
   To investigate the effects of baseline rumination (brooding) and reappraisal on social anxiety,
rumination, and reappraisal measured weekly during CBT, we constructed the following model
with week of assessment as a within-person predictor and baseline brooding and reappraisal as
between-person predictors:

Model 1: Weekly anxiety (or rumination/reappraisal)ij = γ00 + γ01 (baseline brooding) +
 γ02 (baseline reappraisal)+ γ10 (week) + u0j + u1j (week)+ eij

  The fixed effects of interest are γ01 and γ02 , which represent the fixed (or average) effects of
baseline rumination (brooding) and baseline reappraisal on weekly social anxiety, rumination,
and reappraisal levels throughout CBT.
214                          Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2015

  To determine whether weekly rumination and reappraisal were associated with the intensity of
weekly social anxiety during CBT, we constructed the following model with week of assessment,
rumination, and reappraisal and within-person predictors:

Model 2: Weekly social anxietyij = γ00 + γ10 (week) + γ20 (rumination) + γ30 (reappraisal) + u0j
 + u1j (week) + u2j (rumination) + u3j (reappraisal) + eij

   The fixed effects of interest are γ20 and γ30 , which represent the average weekly effects of
rumination and reappraisal, respectively, on social anxiety intensity. The random effects of
interest are the variance parameters (τ22, τ33 elements associated with u2j , u3j , variance–covariance
matrix not displayed) for the rumination and reappraisal slopes, which indicate whether the
average weekly effects of rumination and reappraisal vary across individuals.
   All fixed effects are reported as unstandardized estimates. Effects (baseline rumination [brood-
ing] vs. baseline reappraisal, weekly rumination vs. weekly reappraisal) were compared using
deviance tests; significance was evaluated using the difference in -2 log-likelihood values ob-
tained from a baseline model (with freely estimated effects) and a nested model (with focal
effects constrained to equality), which is chi-square distributed. All multilevel analyses were run
in Mplus v.6.1 (Muthén & Muthén, 1998–2010).

                                               Results
Preliminary Analyses
Socially anxious individuals randomized to CBT or WL groups did not differ significantly on
demographic, rumination (brooding), reappraisal, or weekly variables, ps > .25 (see Table 1).
There were no differences in dropout rates, χ2 (1, N = 75) = 0.05, p = .81, from CBT (n = 6;
15%) and WL (n = 5; 14%). Given no differences were found between immediate and post-WL
CBT completers, we combined the samples for the remainder of the analyses.
  Baseline rumination (brooding) and reappraisal were not related, r(68) = –.10, p > .44.
We examined pre- to post-CBT changes in rumination (brooding) and reappraisal. Repeated
measures ANOVAs demonstrated significant decreases in rumination (brooding), ࢞ = –1.71,
F(1, 37) = 9.71, p = .004, η2 p = .21, and significant increases in reappraisal, ࢞ = 10.78, F(1, 34)
= 31.65, p < .001, η2 p = .48.

Baseline Rumination (Brooding) and Reappraisal Predict Weekly Social Anxiety,
Rumination, and Reappraisal
Controlling for week, greater rumination (brooding) at baseline predicted greater average weekly
levels of social anxiety (γ01 = .97, standard error [SE] = 0.44, p = .03), rumination (γ01 = .94,
SE = 0.47, p = .02), and reappraisal (γ01 = 1.28, SE = .49, p = .008) during 16 weeks of CBT.
However, controlling for week, baseline reappraisal did not predict the average weekly level of
social anxiety (γ02 = -.05, SE = .15, p = .76), rumination (γ02 = -.03, SE = .20, p = .87), or
reappraisal (γ02 = .18, SE = .17, p = .29). All estimates are displayed in Table 2. A deviance
test showed that baseline rumination (brooding) was a stronger predictor of average weekly
social anxiety, ࢞χ2 (1) = 4.84, p = .03, weekly rumination, ࢞χ2 (1) = 3.95, p = .05, and weekly
reappraisal, ࢞χ2 (1) = 4.73, p = .03, than was baseline reappraisal.

Within-Person Associations Among Social Anxiety, Rumination, and Reappraisal
During CBT
During 16 weeks of CBT, greater weekly rumination was associated with greater weekly social
anxiety, γ20 = 0.66, SE = 0.05, p < .001. However, weekly reappraisal was not associated
with weekly social anxiety (γ30 = 0.04, SE = 0.03, p = .10). A deviance test showed that
weekly rumination was a significantly stronger predictor of weekly social anxiety than weekly
reappraisal, ࢞χ2 (1) = 78.38, p < .001.
Rumination, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety                     215

Table 2
Effects of Baseline Rumination (Brooding) and Reappraisal on Weekly Social Anxiety, Rumina-
tion, and Reappraisal

Effect                                  Social anxiety           Rumination          Reappraisal

Fixed effects
  Intercept (γ00 )                      55.67 (8.05)**          60.85 (10.29)**      15.50 (8.91)
  Baseline brooding (γ01 )               0.97 (0.44)*            1.04 (0.58)*         1.28 (0.49)**
  Baseline reappraisal (γ02 )           −0.05 (0.15)            −0.03 (0.20)          0.18 (0.17)
  Week (γ10 )                           −1.45 (0.24)**          −1.45 (0.21)          1.42 (0.22)**
Random effects
  Intercept (τ00 )                    368.07 (77.75)**         263.75 (57.48)**    367.34 (86.49)**
  Week (τ11 )                          −1.36 (0.24)**            1.74 (0.45)**       1.63 (0.54)**
  Intercept-week (τ10 )               −21.60 (6.19)**          −21.48 (6.22)**     −17.26 (5.98)**
  Residual (σ2 )                      151.30 (8.70)**          177.69 (10.46)**    277.52 (15.92)**

Note. Unstandardized estimates are reported; standard errors are in parentheses.
* p < .05. ** p < .01.

                                               Discussion
The goal of the present study was to examine the roles of rumination and reappraisal in gen-
eralized SAD and to better understand how these cognitive processes relate to changes in
social anxiety symptoms during CBT. Consistent with hypotheses, (a) greater baseline rumina-
tion (brooding) predicted greater weekly social anxiety and rumination, and (b) greater weekly
rumination during CBT was associated with greater weekly social anxiety. Contrary to our
hypotheses, greater baseline rumination (brooding) also predicted greater weekly reappraisal.

Toward a Broader Conception of Rumination and Reappraisal
The finding that rumination (brooding) was a stronger predictor than reappraisal and more
closely associated with weekly changes in social anxiety may come as a surprise because changes
in reappraisal appear to be an active ingredient in CBT. However, cross-sectional studies com-
paring the two processes have shown rumination to be more strongly associated than reappraisal
with anxiety (Aldao et al., 2010) and with SAD specifically (D’Avanzato, Joormann, Siemer, &
Gotlib, 2013). Reductions in maladaptive rumination were more informative regarding changes
in social anxiety symptoms than increases in adaptive reappraisal.
   We provided further support for this relationship by showing that rumination was specifically
predictive of social anxiety symptoms, rumination, and reappraisal over time. It was unexpected
that greater baseline rumination (brooding) predicted greater reappraisal. Two potential reasons
for the finding may be that (a) persons with SAD may have a greater need for reappraisal due
to the magnifying lens of rumination, or (b) they may engage in more unproductive attempts
at reappraisal. Further research is needed to replicate these results to have a better sense of the
reasons why rumination (brooding) is associated with increases in reappraisal.
   The process model of emotion regulation (Gross, 2014) provides a helpful context to study
various types of emotion (dys)regulation. Using this framework, rumination can be conceptu-
alized as an attentional deployment strategy, in which individuals focus their attention on a
given stimulus to modify their emotional response. However, this deployment of attention is
maintained or repeated without engaging in cognitive change (Ray, Wilhelm, & Gross, 2008).
Brooding may become particularly dysregulated when individuals are caught in a cognitive cycle
of unproductively mulling over their negative thoughts. Socially anxious individuals’ interpre-
tations of the situation also become increasingly negative over time (Brozovich & Heimberg,
2013). Reappraisal, as discussed earlier, is a generally more adaptive form of cognitive change,
in which individuals aim to change the way they are thinking about a situation to modify their
emotional reactivity to a provocative experience. Viewing brooding as an attentional deployment
216                         Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2015

strategy that may interfere with cognitive change (i.e., reappraisal) elucidates how these emotion
regulation processes may interact with one another.

Conceptual and Clinical Implications for SAD
Among individuals with SAD, rumination has been associated with a number of negative
consequences, such as heightened anxiety (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2011), interpretation biases
(Vassilopoulos & Watkins, 2009), and memory biases (Morgan & Banjeree, 2008). The present
study adds to this literature by showing that brooding, a particularly negative form of rumination,
predicts greater social anxiety and rumination during treatment. But why does rumination play
such an important role in SAD? Rumination is a binding process that occurs before, during,
and after social anxiety-evoking events, thus perpetuating individuals’ anxiety and cognitive
biases (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2013). Brooding can be conceptualized as a particularly sticky
manifestation of rumination that focuses on the negative aspects of one’s behaviors and compares
them to an unrealistic standard. The more individuals examine past information, the more
distorted their memories and interpretations become (Heimberg, Brozovich, & Rapee, 2010).
When an upcoming social anxiety-provoking situation looms on the horizon, the distorted and
negative interpretations are easily accessible, activating greater anticipatory processing of the
next event (Brozovich & Heimberg, 2013). The cycle of brooding continues after the social event
ends.
   Training in cognitive restructuring during CBT allows individuals with SAD to develop a
new way of thinking. CBT helps individuals to increase reappraisal, which directs their attention
to entertain alternative perspectives and challenges the veracity of their thoughts. This occurs
before, during, and after social situations. At the same time, CBT helps individuals increase
exposure to feared situations. In the present study, rumination (brooding) predicted social
anxiety over the course of 4 months of CBT above and beyond reappraisal. It may be that CBT
freed individuals’ attention from brooding, thereby helping to reduce social anxiety.
   CBT has clearly been effective in diminishing rumination and increasing reappraisal in social
anxiety in the present RCT. A handful of other studies have also shown that rumination decreases
during CBT (e.g., Price & Anderson, 2011). Our study provides evidence that baseline rumination
(brooding) predicts social anxiety and rumination and changes in rumination during treatment
inform us about social anxiety symptom changes. If future studies substantiate our findings,
it may be useful for clinicians to measure rumination during the course of treatment to better
assess improvement in ruminative habits alongside changes in social anxiety.

Limitations and Future Directions
The present study examined rumination and reappraisal, as well as their associations with social
anxiety during CBT for SAD. We collected socially anxious individuals’ self-reports of their
ruminative tendencies using the brooding subscale of the RRS before CBT. The present study
is the first to use the RRS to examine brooding in SAD during an RCT of CBT. The RRS is a
psychometrically sound instrument measuring rumination (Treynor et al., 2003). However, the
RRS was not designed to measure rumination in social anxiety and has often been criticized
for overlapping with symptoms of depression. We utilized the brooding subscale, which is less
influenced by depression-related items; yet we did not include other rumination measures that
have been developed specifically for social anxiety (e.g., Post-Event Processing Questionnaire;
Rachman, Gruter-Andrew, & Shafran, 2000). Future studies would benefit from including post-
event processing measures alongside the RRS to examine rumination and how it specifically
relates to social anxiety after social situations.
    Our focus in this study was on a sample of individuals with generalized SAD with limited
comorbidity. This research strategy allowed us to isolate the role of rumination (brooding) and
reappraisal in a relatively homogeneous clinical context. However, as clinical science moves
toward a more transdiagnostic approach (Nolen-Hoeksema & Watkins, 2011), it is essential to
examine maladaptive mechanisms that may exacerbate symptoms across disorders, as well as
adaptive ones that may ameliorate symptoms.
Rumination, Reappraisal, and Social Anxiety                                 217

   Although we focused solely on Nolen-Hoeksema’s conceptualization of maladaptive rumina-
tion (brooding), Watkins (2008) points out that rumination has adaptive purposes and conse-
quences as well. An interesting area to pursue may be to measure individuals’ ruminative habits
using scales that capture maladaptive and adaptive components and how these may fluctuate
within the same individual after receiving treatment for social anxiety or other diagnoses. There
are also other facets to reappraisal. For example, reappraisal self-efficacy, or one’s cognitive
evaluation of one’s ability to implement reappraisal, has proven to affect treatment response in
social anxiety both immediately after and 1 year after treatment (Goldin et al., 2012). Thus,
people’s appraisals of how successful they are at using a given emotion regulation strategy may
affect frequency of use as well. These are additional variables to consider when studying any
emotion regulation strategy.

                                                References
Aderka, I. M., Hofmann, S. G., Nickerson, A., Hermesh, H., Gilboa-Schectman, E., & Marom, S.
   (2012). Functional impairment in social anxiety disorder. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 26, 393–400.
   doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2012.01.003
Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathol-
   ogy: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.).
   Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Press.
American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.).
   Washington, DC: Author.
Brown, T. A., Di Nardo, P. A., Lehman, C. L., & Campbell, L. A. (2001). Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety
   and mood disorders: Implications for the classification of emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal
   Psychology, 110, 49–58. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.110.1.49
Brozovich, F., & Heimberg, R. G. (2008). An analysis of post-event processing in social anxiety disorder.
   Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 891–903. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.01.002
Brozovich, F., & Heimberg, R. G. (2011). The relationship of post-event processing to self-evaluation of
   performance in social anxiety. Behavior Therapy, 42, 224–235. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2010.08.005
Brozovich, F., & Heimberg, R. G. (2013). Mental imagery and post-event processing in anticipation of a
   speech performance among socially anxious individuals. Behavior Therapy, 44, 701–716.
Clark, D. M., & Wells, A. (1995). A cognitive model of social phobia. In R. G. Heimberg, M. Liebowitz,
   D. A. Hope, & F. R. Schneier (Eds.), Social phobia: Diagnosis, assessment, and treatment (pp. 69–93).
   New York: Guilford Press.
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Psychology
   Press.
D’Avanzato, C., Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Emotion regulation in depression and
   anxiety: Examining diagnostic specificity and stability of strategy use. Cognitive Therapy and Research,
   37, 968–980. doi:10.1007/s10608-013-9537-0
DiNardo, P. A., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. (1994). Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for DSM-IV:
   Lifetime version (ADIS-IV-L). New York: Oxford University Press.
Efron, B. (1971). Forcing a sequential experiment to be balanced. Biometrika, 58, 403–417.
Goldin, P. R., Manber-Ball, T., Werner, K., Heimberg, R. G., & Gross, J. J. (2009). Neural mechanisms
   of cognitive reappraisal of negative self-beliefs in social anxiety disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 66,
   1091–1099. doi:10.1016/jbiopsych.2009.07.014
Goldin, P. R., Ziv, M., Jazaieri, H., Werner, K., Kraemer, H., Heimberg, R. G., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Cognitive
   reappraisal self-efficacy mediates the effects of individual cognitive-behavioral therapy for social anxiety
   disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80, 1034–1040. doi:10.1037/a0028555
Gross, J. J. (2014). Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations. In J.J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook
   of emotion regulation (2nd ed., pp. 3–20). New York: Guilford Press.
Gross, J. J., & John, O. P. (2003). Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications
   for affect, relationships, and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 348–362.
Heimberg, R. G., Brozovich, F., & Rapee, R. (2010). An integrated cognitive-behavioral model of social
   anxiety. In S. G. Hofmann & P. M. Di Bartolo (Eds.), Social anxiety: Clinical, developmental and social
218                           Journal of Clinical Psychology, March 2015

   perspectives (2nd ed., pp. 395–422). New York: Academic Press. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-375096-9.00015-
   8
Hope, D. A., Heimberg, R. G., Juster, J., & Turk, C. L. (2000). Managing social anxiety: A cognitive-
  behavioral approach (Client workbook, 1st edition). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hope, D. A., Heimberg, R. G., & Turk, C. L. (2006). Managing social anxiety: A cognitive-behavioral
  therapy approach (Therapist Guide, 1st ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.
Hope, D., VanDyke, M., Heimberg, R. G., Turk, C. L., & Fresco, D. M. (2001). Cognitive-behavioral
  therapy for social anxiety disorder: Therapist adherence scale. [Available from Richard G. Heimberg,
  Adult Anxiety Clinic of Temple University, 1701 North 13th Street, Philadelphia, PA 19122–6085.]
Hox, J. (2002). Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications. Mahwah, NY: Erlbaum.
Joormann, J., Dkane, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2006). Adaptive and maladaptive components of rumi-
   nation? Diagnostic specificity and relation to depressive biases. Behavior Therapy, 37, 269–280.
   doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.01.002
Morgan, J., & Banerjee, R. (2008). Post-event processing and autobiographical memory in social anxiety:
   The influence of negative feedback and rumination. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 1190–1204.
   doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2008.01.001
Moscovitch, D. A., Gavric, D. L., Senn, J. M., Santesso, D. L., Miskovic, V., Schmidt, L.A., . . . Antony,
   M. M. (2012). Changes in judgment biases and use of emotion regulation strategies during cognitive-
   behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder: Distinguishing treatment responders and nonresponders.
   Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36, 261–271. doi:10.1007/s10608-011-9371-1
Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2010). Mplus user’s guide. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1991). Responses to depression and their effects on the duration of depressive episodes.
  Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 569–582.
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A heuristic for developing transdiagnostic models of psy-
  chopathology: Explaining multifinality and divergent trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science,
  6, 589–609. doi:10.1177/1745691611419672
Pearson, K. A., Watkins, E. R., Kuyken, W., & Mullan, E. G. (2010). The psychosocial context of
   depressive rumination: Ruminative brooding predicts diminished relationship satisfaction in individ-
   uals with a history of past major depression. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49, 275–280.
   doi:10.1348/014466509X480553
Price, M., & Anderson, P. L. (2011). The impact of cognitive-behavioral therapy on post-event pro-
    cessing among those with social anxiety disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 132–137.
    doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.006
Rachman, S., Gruter-Andrew, J., & Shafran, R. (2000). Post-event processing in social anxiety. Behaviour
    Research and Therapy, 38, 611–617. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00089-3
Rapee, R. M., & Heimberg, R. G. (1997). A cognitive-behavioral model of anxiety in social phobia. Behaviour
    Research and Therapy, 35, 741–756.
Ray, R. D., Wilhelm, F. H., & Gross, J. J. (2008). All in the mind’s eye? Anger rumination and reappraisal.
   Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 133–145. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.133
Snijders, T. A. B., & Bosker, R. (1999). Multilevel modeling: An introduction to basic and advanced
   multilevel modeling. London: Sage Publications.
Trew, J. L., & Alden, L. E. (2009). Predicting anger in social anxiety: The mediating role of rumination.
   Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47, 1079–1084. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2009.07.019
Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric anal-
   ysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259. doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561
Vassilopoulos, S. P., & Watkins, E. (2009). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus: A pilot study
   with individuals high and low in fear of negative evaluation. Behavior Therapy, 40, 181–189.
   doi:10.1016/j.beth.2008.05.003
Watkins, E. R. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological Bulletin, 134,
   163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163
Watkins, E. R. (2009). Depressive rumination and comorbidity: Evidence for brooding as a trans-
  diagnostic process. Journal of Rational-Emotive & Cognitive-Behavior Therapy, 27.3, 160–175.
  doi:10.1007/s10942-009-0098-9
Werner, K. H., Goldin, P. R., Ball, T. M., Heimberg, R. G., & Gross, J. J. (2011). Assessing emotion
  regulation in social anxiety disorder: The Emotion Regulation Interview. Journal of Psychopathology
  and Behavioral Assessment, 33, 346–354. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9225-x
You can also read