Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan Summary In this article, developmental psychologists Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan examine the relation between parenting practices and socioeconomic gaps in child outcomes. They document substantial differences between richer and poorer families, including growing gaps in parental engagement and time use. These gaps matter: the fact that children born to lower-income, less-educated parents are less likely to spend quality time with their parents only compounds their relative economic disadvantage. Evidence suggests that disadvantaged parents want to do many of the same things that higher-income parents do, such as reading to their children and engaging them in educational experiences like trips to parks and museums. But they’re nonetheless less likely to do those things. The authors consider a number of explanations for this discrepancy. One important contributing factor, Kalil and Ryan write, appears to be financial strain and family stress, both of which can impede parents’ emotional and cognitive functioning in ways that make it harder for them to interact with young children in intellectually stimulating and emotionally nurturing ways. The authors conclude with a discussion of the types of policies and programs that might narrow income-based parenting gaps. They find encouraging evidence that relatively low- cost, light-touch behavioral interventions could help parents overcome the cognitive biases that may prevent them from using certain beneficial parenting practices. www.futureofchildren.org Ariel Kalil is a professor in the Harris School of Public Policy at the University of Chicago, where she directs the Center for Human Potential and Public Policy and co-directs the Behavioral Insights and Parenting Lab. Rebecca Ryan is a Provost’s Distinguished Associate Professor in the Department of Psychology at Georgetown University. Richard Reeves of the Brookings Institution reviewed and critiqued a draft of this article. VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 29
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan S ocioeconomic status is correlated way children develop. But we also recognize across generations. In the United that the vast majority of empirical evidence States, 43 percent of adults who supporting these theoretical mechanisms is were raised in the poorest fifth correlational.5 This correlational research of the income distribution now consistently finds that average differences have incomes in the poorest fifth, and 70 in parenting behavior predict the cognitive, percent have incomes in the poorest half. academic, and behavioral outcomes that Likewise, among adults raised in the richest presage adult success.6 Although it’s not fifth of the income distribution, 40 percent our purpose here to describe the plausibly have incomes in the richest fifth and 53 causal links between parenting practices and percent have incomes in the richest half.1 child development outcomes, more recent Many factors influence this intergenerational studies relying on experimental and quasi- correlation, but evidence suggests that experimental methods do find evidence of parenting practices play a crucial role. such links. For example, recent research These include doing enriching activities finds that the amount of time parents spend with children, getting involved in their with their children has a direct and plausibly schoolwork, providing educational materials, causal effect on the children’s cognitive test and exhibiting warmth and patience. Parental scores.7 behavior interpreted in this way probably Here, we focus on specific parenting accounts for around half of the variance in practices that have been linked empirically adult economic outcomes, and therefore and theoretically to better child development, contributes significantly to a country’s and we explore how those practices vary intergenerational mobility.2 by socioeconomic status (SES). We review Across disciplines, dozens of studies studies that characterize SES in terms of have found differences in these types of family income and also parental education. behaviors across rich and poor parents Specifically, we provide an overview of and demonstrated how these differences what scholars know about the differences in matter to children’s success as adults. parenting behavior by SES that contribute Among other things, richer parents—both to differences in children’s outcomes by mothers and fathers—spend more time in SES. We also examine the theoretical educational activities with their children, origins of these differences in parenting produce more cognitively stimulating home behavior. The origins are interrelated and learning environments, and are more likely wide-ranging: they include socioeconomic to read and do math-related activities with and demographic factors, such as financial their children.3 Parents also differ by family constraints and parental work schedules; background in their discipline strategies— opportunities and constraints in the parents’ those with lower incomes and less education environment, such as access to information are more likely to use harsh, physical and exposure to stress and violence; and discipline with children than their richer and “cultural” factors, such as norms, beliefs and more educated counterparts.4 expectations, habits, and values. We don’t try to draw a bright line between so-called In this article, we take as a given that the way cultural factors and others; rather, we believe parents behave with children influences the that many factors that are often thought of as 30 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes cultural are actually the result of other, more children’s development. Developmental readily quantifiable contextual factors, such psychology distinguishes these parenting as stress, money, information, and parents’ behaviors along two key dimensions: the reactions to those things. This article reviews level of cognitive stimulation and the quality how contextual factors differ (or not) in of emotional support. Cognitive stimulation meaningful ways between low- and high- includes enriching behaviors like reading SES parents, and whether and how those and other literacy activities, doing arts and differences relate to parenting behavior. crafts, and discussing math concepts. Positive socioemotional interactions involve parental warmth and consistency and the absence of harsh discipline or physical punishment. Dozens of studies across On average, research shows, parents with disciplines have demonstrated more education and income engage in that richer and poorer more cognitive stimulation with their young children, interact with greater warmth and parents interact with their consistency, and use harsh discipline less children in different ways, often than do parents with less education and and that these differences income. influence children’s Differences in Cognitive Stimulation development. Among studies showing that higher- SES parents engage in more cognitively stimulating activities in terms of both We conclude by discussing the efficacy of quantity and quality than their lower-SES programs and policies designed to narrow peers do, some of strongest evidence comes SES-based differences in parenting behavior, from time diaries. The most replicated and we suggest directions for promising finding is that mothers and fathers with more policy and programmatic interventions based education and income spend more time in on this review. Although parental behavior educational activities with their children matters far beyond the early childhood years, than do lower-SES parents.9 The authors of here we focus on early childhood, given the this article, Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan, primacy of parental influence during this along with colleague Michael Corey, have developmental stage and the speed of early also shown that highly educated mothers childhood brain development, both of which and fathers are more efficient in their provide the foundation for cognitive and parental time investments—they tailor emotional skills over the life course.8 their activities to children’s developmental Differences in Parenting Practices stages.10 With respect to total childcare time, by Socioeconomic Status the educational gradient is most apparent in households with the youngest children.11 Dozens of studies across disciplines have Together, these findings suggest that higher- demonstrated that richer and poorer parents SES parents aren’t only investing more time interact with their children in different in their children’s development, but they ways, and that these differences influence may also have better assimilated the message VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 31
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan that parental investments in early childhood heard by children in higher- versus lower- are key ingredients in children’s long-run SES families comes from words spoken success.12 directly to the children, not words said when children are present, and that the language Studies that draw on stylized measures of used in higher-SES homes is more diverse the frequency with which parents engage and responsive to children’s speech than in enriching activities tell a similar story. that in lower-SES homes.17 This SES-based When asked how often per week or month difference in linguistic environments could they engage in reading, math, or other plausibly contribute to SES-based gaps in enriching activities at home, higher-SES children’s early language skills, especially parents are more likely to report reading given the robust evidence linking the to and doing math-related activities with quantity and quality of parents’ speech to their children; they’re also more likely to young children to children’s early language provide the materials, such as books, puzzles, development.18 and games, with which to engage in those activities.13 Over the past 30 years, in fact, Differences in Emotional Support high-SES parents have consistently engaged in a wide range of enriching activities in As we’ve said, parents differ by SES not and outside the home—such as reading to only in the quality and quantity of cognitive children and taking them to the library or a stimulation they offer children, but also in museum—far more often than their lower- the level of emotional support they provide. SES counterparts.14 Parental sensitivity—defined as the ability to perceive children’s signals, interpret these One of the best-known SES-based signals correctly, and respond promptly and differences in cognitive stimulation comes appropriately—has been theoretically and from research on language stimulation empirically linked with children’s emotional of young children. Higher-SES parents and behavioral outcomes, including self- use greater language stimulation when regulation, social functioning, and early interacting with children than do their cognitive skills.19 Mothers living in poverty lower-SES counterparts.15 A famous example display less sensitivity during interactions of this difference comes from a study by with their babies than do their higher-SES Betty Hart and Todd Risley, who intensively counterparts, and in descriptive analyses observed the language patterns of 42 families these differences explain gaps in children’s with young children.16 They found that in early language outcomes and behavior professional families, children heard an problems.20 average of 2,153 words per hour; in working- class families, the number was 1,251 words More broadly, better-off parents tend to per hour; and in welfare-recipient families, display more of what’s called an authoritative it was only 616 words per hour. By age four, (versus authoritarian) parenting style a child in a welfare-recipient family could than lower-SES parents do. Authoritative have heard 32 million fewer words than a parenting describes a broad style of classmate in a professional family. More interacting in which parents place high recent studies have clarified that the bulk demands on children but also use high of the difference in the number of words levels of warmth and responsiveness. 32 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes Authoritarian parenting, by contrast, is punishment, which includes spanking, characterized by strict limits on children and hitting with objects, and other actions that little warmth or dialogue, and punishment intentionally cause physical pain, is associated tends to be harsh.21 Studies have found that with an authoritarian parenting style, whereas parents—both mothers and fathers—with nonphysical discipline strategies such as more education are more likely to use an time-outs and explanations for desirable authoritative style than less-educated parents, behaviors are associated with an authoritative who are likelier to use either an authoritarian style.25 Research shows that lower-SES style or a permissive style (characterized parents spank and use other forms of physical by “low demands coupled with high levels discipline more often than higher-SES of warmth and responsiveness”), a pattern parents do, whereas higher-SES families we see within racial and ethnic groups and are more likely to use discipline strategies in cross-country comparisons.22 Supporting that include reasoning and promote child these broad differences in style, studies have autonomy.26 Studies show links between also shown that lower-income parents use corporal punishment, such as spanking, and a more directives and prohibitions in speech host of adverse cognitive and socioemotional with children than their middle-income child outcomes.27 Perhaps it’s not surprising, counterparts do.23 Finally, in a large national then, that in descriptive analyses these sample, researchers saw a significant negative disciplinary differences explain a meaningful correlation between punitive behavior (such proportion of SES-based differences as yelling and hitting) and income.24 in children’s outcomes, particularly socioemotional ones.28 Given the well-documented Summary links between … parenting Decades of research have demonstrated that behaviors and children’s lower- and higher-SES parents differ not just in the ways they raise their children, but skills, it’s reasonable to also in precisely the behaviors that predict hypothesize that SES-based children’s cognitive and socioemotional differences in parenting skills. Given the well-documented links behaviors contribute to between these parenting behaviors and children’s skills, it’s reasonable to hypothesize the intergenerational that SES-based differences in parenting transmission of economic behaviors contribute to the intergenerational status. transmission of economic status. In the next section, we review research on some possible origins of these parenting Discipline strategies are a central component differences, and assess the evidence in of socioemotional interaction between support of each mechanism. Understanding parents and children and a key facet of how these parenting gaps arise is essential to the difference between authoritative determining an effective policy and program and authoritarian parenting. Corporal response that could narrow them. VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 33
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan Mechanisms Underlying SES- The best evidence on differences in money Based Gaps in Parenting Practices spent on children across the socioeconomic distribution comes from two studies by The wide-ranging scholarship on SES- Emory University sociologist Sabino based differences in parenting practices Kornrich, using data from the Consumer offers many potential origins for these Expenditure Survey. (This survey, conducted differences. Different fields (such as by the Bureau of Labor Statistics, provides economics, sociology, psychology, and data on the expenditures, income, and neuroscience) stress different potential demographic characteristics of US mechanisms. Below, we distinguish five of consumers.) Kornrich and his colleague these mechanisms and present evidence that Frank Furstenberg found not only that each one might plausibly help explain SES- parents at the top of the income distribution based parenting gaps. In doing so, we aim to spend more on children’s enrichment than illuminate promising targets for policy and lower-income parents do, but also that the programmatic intervention to narrow SES- difference in real dollars has increased based gaps in parenting. substantially since the 1970s.30 This spending gap has grown despite the fact that parents at Financial Constraints all income levels are devoting an increasing share of their income to children, and that The most obvious reason higher-SES the lowest-income parents spend the largest parents might use different parenting share. Kornrich extended the analysis practices than their lower-SES counterparts by examining income-based inequality is that they simply have more money to in parental spending on young children buy the materials and experiences that specifically over the period from 1972 to enhance child wellbeing. This mechanism 2010.31 He found increased spending among may sound tautological—surely parents parents at the top of the income distribution who have more money to spend on their but little change among those at the bottom. children do so. Yet some parents may choose Much of the increase derived from additional to spend discretionary income on their own spending on childcare. But spending on leisure and consumption rather than on enrichment goods (such as books, toys, their children.29 With respect to cognitive games, and fees for activities) also grew stimulation, child-related expenditures substantially among higher-income families include materials to enhance time with during this time, and grew not at all among children—such as books, toys, and games— lower-income families. as well as costly experiences such as dance, music, and sports lessons; tutoring; and These studies, combined with other research museum visits and artistic performances. that finds differences in the presence Differences in spending on these types of books, toys, and games in lower- and of enrichment goods could directly affect higher-income families’ homes, suggest that children’s development by enhancing the unequal spending on children undoubtedly quantity and quality of their cognitive explains some of the SES-based differences stimulation, but could also affect parents’ in parenting practices.32 Still, in a recent ability to invest time in enhancing their study of parent-child reading time in low- children’s development. income Chicago families, almost no parents 34 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes reported that they failed to read to their being accessible to their children but largely children because they lacked the appropriate engaged in housework or leisure activities.37 books.33 Parents can also do many activities Of course, it’s challenging to separate time with children that are enriching but not availability from family structure: low-income necessarily costly—such as talking to parents are far likelier to be single parents, them, telling stories, and playing games. with less economic and social support to Thus, although differences in children’s lighten the competing demands of household experiences by SES surely arise in part tasks than married mothers. (For more about from differences in parents’ ability to pay single parents and other aspects of family for enrichment, financial constraints don’t structure, see Melanie Wasserman’s article entirely explain the gap. in this issue.38) Nonetheless, in a cross- national comparison study, highly educated Time Constraints mothers in many developed countries spent Another possible reason that lower- more time than less-educated mothers in SES parents engage in fewer cognitively primary child investment activities—even stimulating activities with children, and in Norway, where universal family policies spend less time with them in general, is are designed to equalize resources across that these parents simply have less time parents.39 The authors interpreted their to spare. Research on work hours and findings as suggesting that the differences schedules shows that lower-SES parents are between more- and less-educated mothers in likelier than higher-SES parents to work time spent with children more likely reflect unpredictable and nonstandard hours.34 different beliefs about parenting rather than All else being equal, it’s hard for parents time constraints. to engage in developmentally stimulating In sum, it isn’t clear how much of the SES- activities with children when they’re based difference in time investment in working during prime “investment” time children stems from differences in time (that is, weekends and evenings). Several available versus parents’ decisions to allocate quantitative studies show that the SES- available time to their children. These based gaps in time investment remain large decisions may be shaped by information, even when accounting for other family values, and preferences—topics we turn to differences, such as employment hours, next. but few researchers have explored how the timing or regularity of work hours might Information, Values, and Preferences explain SES-based differences in parents’ about Parenting and Child Development time investment.35 That said, high-SES parents (especially mothers) tend to work Given that lower-SES parents may invest more hours than lower-SES parents and less time in children not just because they have less discretionary time—but still spend have less time to spend but rather because more time with their children.36 This stems they spend the time they have differently, it’s from fact that higher-SES parents (especially reasonable to hypothesize that SES-based mothers) spend more of their childcare time differences in this area stem in part from primarily engaged in activities, while lower- differences in information on, values about, SES mothers tend to spend childcare time or preferences for spending time doing VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 35
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan enriching activities with children. We could important.41 Moreover, for each of the skills, extend this hypothesis to cover emotionally the proportion of parents who said the skill supportive behaviors as well: perhaps lower- was important was greater in the lowest fifth SES parents have less information about how of SES than in the highest fifth. It’s unlikely, warm, sensitive parent-child interactions then, that the large differences by SES in the can benefit children’s socioemotional actual skills of children entering kindergarten development—or perhaps they have weaker arise because disadvantaged parents lack preferences for such interactions. To be information about the importance of those sure, information, values, and preferences skills. are different concepts: information generally refers to parents’ knowledge of child development and the activities that US parents at all income promote it; values reflect parents’ goals for their children and their ideal traits; and levels believe it’s important preferences refer to taste factors that may for children to develop skills influence parental behavior, such as level of that will prepare them for happiness, degree of satisfaction, or utility.40 success in school and life; To understand how these factors drive SES-based differences in parental behavior, they also share similar ideas we need strong evidence that information, about the values they wish to values, and preferences differ by SES. instill in their children. But most recent evidence suggests that US parents at all income levels believe it’s important for children to develop skills that Another key aspect of the parental belief will prepare them for success in school and system is terminal values for children—the life; they also share similar ideas about the characteristics parents believe they must values they wish to instill in their children. instill in children to prepare them for life.42 For example, one group of researchers The concept of values is often invoked in examined data from the 1998 and 2010 Early discussions of “cultural beliefs” as they Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS—a relate to parenting: researchers have posited national study by the National Center for for decades that the difference in terminal Education Statistics that examines child values among parents at different points in development, school readiness, and early the income or education distribution is one school experiences). Their goal was to source of the intergenerational persistence of estimate the share of parents who rated social class.43 Historically, high-SES parents various early childhood kindergarten have valued “independent thinking” and readiness skills—such as counting to “self-direction” more than low-SES parents 20, knowing the letters of the alphabet, do, whereas low-SES parents have put more and sitting still—as “very important” or value on “obedience” and “conformity.” “essential.” The researchers found an Theoretically, differences in parental values increase between 1998 and 2010 in the help account for the reproduction of social proportion of parents in the lowest fifth of class partly because of the myriad ways the SES distribution who rated these skills as these differences influence how parents 36 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes prepare their children for their academic Income may also shape the extent to which and professional futures. But our recent parents view the time they allocate to their research with our colleagues Caitlin Hines children as direct utility versus investment and Kathleen Ziol-Guest shows that rich and utility. Economists have long thought that poor parents’ ideas about the characteristics highly educated parents, more so than less- needed for children to succeed in life (such educated parents, view time with children as working hard, being helpful, and thinking as an “investment behavior,” a means to for oneself) have converged substantially in increase children’s future human capital.48 the past three decades. In fact, in 2016 we This framework may help explain why found no significant differences in parents’ highly educated parents spend more time espoused values by education or income.44 in childcare than less-educated parents Using a nationally representative survey, who work the same hours and have as many another study similarly found no differences children. But the same theories suggest by parental education in contemporary that highly educated parents might spend parenting standards: parents of all social relatively more time with their children backgrounds strongly endorsed time- because they derive more enjoyment from intensive, child-centered parenting as the the activity. Using data from the 2010–13 optimal parenting style (what sociologist American Time Use Survey Well-Being Annette Lareau labeled “concerted Module (a national study conducted by cultivation”), whereas parents of all social the Bureau of Labor Statistics that links backgrounds showed little support for a less self-reported wellbeing information to intensive, adult-centered parenting style individuals’ activities and time-use patterns), (which Lareau labeled “natural growth”).45 Kalil and colleagues examined mothers’ reports of how they feel during childcare and Even though all parents have similar in other activities, seeking empirical evidence aspirations for their children’s development pertaining to economic theories of time and readiness for school, lower-SES parents allocation.49 For all mothers, spending time in might expect a lower return from their childcare was associated with higher positive investments in their children. But researchers feelings than was spending time in other have shown that low-income parents do activities. This finding offers no support for expect a positive return from the time they the hypothesis that highly educated mothers spend in educational activities with their enjoy childcare more than their less-educated children; in one study done in Colombia, counterparts do. among very low-income households, parents’ beliefs predicted investment in young In sum, research provides only mixed children.46 Furthermore, a survey of parents evidence that information, values, or of school-aged children in England found no preferences drive differences in parenting SES-based difference in expected returns to behavior across the socioeconomic time or money invested in children. But some distribution. Compared to higher-SES research suggests that although all parents parents, lower-SES parents may have less expect high returns on their investment in nuanced ideas about how to promote child child development, lower-SES parents expect development and may underestimate the relatively lower returns than their higher-SES benefits of time spent promoting child peers do.47 development. But high- and low-SES parents VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 37
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan alike understand the importance and value probability of being diagnosed with clinical of enriching behavior with children, such as depression.54 reading, and they appear to enjoy this time in equal measure. According to the family stress model, this psychological distress spills over into all Family and Environmental Stress family relationships. As couples struggle to make ends meet, and as their depression, Sociologist Glen Elder developed the family anxiety, and parenting stress increase, stress model to explain how economic loss their interactions with each other and with influenced parent and child wellbeing their children become more hostile and during the Great Depression.50 According conflicted.55 Evidence of these associations to this perspective, low-income families face includes a study using data from the Panel significant economic pressure as they struggle Study of Income Dynamics (a household to pay bills and purchase important goods survey of family income, employment, and services, and this economic pressure, health, and wellbeing that follows its coupled with other stressful events more subjects over time). Kalil and her colleague prevalent in the lives of low-income families, Patrick Wightman found that parental job causes poor parents to suffer psychological loss, particularly that of the father, was distress that can disrupt parent-child associated with increases in marital conflict interactions.51 Although the model was and interpersonal violence; this finding is developed to explain the impact of economic replicated in other studies.56 In turn, parents’ loss on family wellbeing, it has since been psychological distress and conflict with one applied to the way chronic economic strain, another are linked with parenting practices and poverty in particular, can undermine that are on average more punitive, harsh, parenting quality.52 inconsistent, and detached, as well as less nurturing, stimulating, and responsive to Research has substantiated many of the children’s needs.57 Associations that link hypothetical links in the family stress economic strain to psychological stress to model—at least in observational studies. In disrupted parenting have been documented low-income families, parents and children consistently in studies of early child alike experience more daily stress than their development.58 higher-income peers do, and low-income parents report higher levels of parenting In recent years, the family stress model’s stress and depression.53 Specifically, poor focus on environmental conditions and mothers of infants are two and a half times parents’ mental health has broadened to more likely to report being depressed include our understanding of how stress than nonpoor mothers. These studies affects neurobiological and cognitive only establish correlations among poverty, functioning. Specifically, studies show stress, and parents’ wellbeing. But more that in the context of scarcity, including a causal evidence comes from one study’s lack of money, parents are more likely to finding that decreases in income among make decisions that emphasize short-term parents, particularly those who are already rather than long-term gains.59 This greatly at low income levels, predicted increases diminishes the possibilities for purposeful, in mothers’ depressive symptoms and their goal-directed parenting. Taken together with 38 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes the scholarship on the family stress model, decisions, for a variety of reasons—such these growing areas of research suggest that as differences in stress, in the composition financial strain can alter parents’ emotional of their social networks, or in parents’ own and cognitive functioning in ways that upbringing or experiences. Or they may undermine their ability to be cognitively experience the same heuristic differently, stimulating and emotionally sensitive with which might also result in different patterns young children. of decision-making. Here we describe two potentially important characteristics Cognitive Biases and Decision-Making that make parenting especially susceptible to cognitive biases and to differential It seems that disadvantaged parents want to adaptations to biases by parental SES. do many of the same things that better-off parents do, especially activities associated Present bias. For many parenting decisions, with more positive child outcomes like the payoff doesn’t materialize until years into reading aloud and going on educational the future. Deciding to spend money and outings. However, they’re less likely to time on schooling, extracurricular activities, actually do those things. That is, we see a health-promoting behaviors, and other wider gap in disadvantaged families between activities meant to improve child outcomes is what parents aspire to do and what they do in much like making financial investments with practice. Researchers in behavioral science uncertain returns. Research suggests that and behavioral economics have described a people systematically put too much weight set of cognitive biases that may give rise to on present outcomes as opposed to future this gap between knowing and doing. outcomes, which often leads to suboptional choices.61 Present bias can mean that parents prioritize spending their time on activities that provide immediate gratification rather People systematically put than investing that time in their children. too much weight on present For example, even if parents believe in outcomes as opposed to future the value of reading, have books at home, and understand the connection between outcomes, which often leads parents’ reading aloud and children’s skill to suboptimal choices. development, they may be less likely to read to their children routinely because the temptation to do something else in the Like many other decisions, parenting moment overcomes the commitment to decisions are complex. This fact constrains invest time in an activity whose payoff lies parents’ capacity to make optimal decisions, in a distant and uncertain future. Scholars simply because human judgment can’t readily have reached no consensus on what causes master the complexity of parenting. Thus differences in time preference. Many early parents are prone to relying on heuristics sociology studies provide observational (cognitive shortcuts) to simplify their evidence that time preference is culturally decisions and make them “computationally acquired.62 Economists Gary Becker and cheap.”60 Lower- and higher-SES parents Casey Mulligan proposed in 1997 that the may use different heuristics in making more financial resources people have to VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 39
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan imagine the future, the less future value behaviors are primarily learned from one’s they’re willing to give up for present value own parents, relatives, and friends.65 (that is, they are more patient, or future- focused). Empirical evidence supports this Automaticity reduces cognitive demands, hypothesis.63 More recently, Priyanka Joshi leads to rapid responses, and is useful for and Nathanael Fast claimed that power many parenting situations. But it can also makes people feel more connected to their create barriers to eliminating adverse parent futures, which in turn results in less present behaviors (such as yelling at or hitting a child, bias (that is, more patience). To the extent or forgoing toothbrushing). Higher- and that income can be perceived as power, lower-SES parents may have the same goals this could help explain why low-income for their children; they may even have the individuals experience present bias.64 same information about how to achieve those goals. But parenting behaviors are correlated Automaticity bias. Parenting often requires across generations. Thus, higher- and lower- quick, on-the-spot decisions. When a child SES parents may have different parenting runs toward a busy street, a parent must habits because of the differences in their react, not contemplate. When a child screams own upbringings in different socioeconomic in the checkout lane because a parent has circumstances.66 In this way, automaticity bias turned down a request for candy, the parent can help reinforce SES-based differences seldom has time to reflect on what to do. The in the cognitive stimulation and emotional need to act quickly results in automaticity, support provided by parents. meaning a response with minimal cognitive processing. Automaticity is a useful heuristic Summary that reduces cognitive load. An automatic The evidence reviewed above suggests that response can be beneficial if it’s efficacious, neither time constraints nor differences in but costly when it’s not. Because automatic parental preferences or beliefs likely explain responses can be likened to habits, and habits much of the variation in parenting behavior are hard to break, ineffective automatic by SES. Differences in how much money responses can lead to ineffective parenting. is available to high- and low-SES parents Automaticity comes from learning, repetition, undoubtedly affect differences in parents’ and practice. The automatic behaviors material investments in their children, but parents adopt are likely to depend on their money isn’t likely to explain the differences own experiences. Behaviors repeatedly in how parents spend time with children. We observed or experienced as a child can easily do see consistent evidence that lower- and become default behaviors in adulthood. An higher-SES parents interact with and invest adult whose parents always spanked him in children differently because low-SES when he misbehaved as a child is more likely parents undergo more daily and often “toxic” to “automatically” spank his own children in stress than higher-income parents do. That response to bad behavior, giving little thought type of stress increases parents’ depression to alternative kinds of discipline. We don’t and anxiety and can undermine their have a lot of evidence about how people learn cognitive ability to focus on long-term goals to be parents, but what we do have (usually rather than short-term ones. Recent research from small surveys) suggests that parenting has also found that the economic context 40 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes of parenting itself, whatever its impact on recipients compared to nonrecipients find mental health, is associated with cognitive that EITC recipients are likely spend their biases in parents that may encourage them to check on durable goods like cars or home focus on present versus future gains and to appliances, or to pay off debts, and not on rely on habits rather than conscious problem- child-related expenditures per se.67 However, solving when making parenting decisions. a recent study found that receiving the EITC during the spring of a child’s senior year Policies and Programs to Narrow in high school boosted the likelihood that the Gap low-income students would enroll in college, What types of policies and programs suggesting that the additional money may would most effectively narrow SES- sometimes go to education.68 Taken together, based parenting gaps? Below, we review this research suggests that providing more research about programs that aim to money isn’t likely to alter the time or money improve parenting behavior by targeting spent by parents on daily child enrichment precisely the mechanisms discussed above. activities, but it might increase parental Wherever possible, we review evidence investment in larger expenditures like college from randomized controlled trials or from enrollment. quasi-experimental studies (which compare Another source of information on how treatment groups and control groups formed a sudden change in income might affect by means other than random assignment, families comes from a study on the effect of such as a policy change). Otherwise, we a windfall from casino earnings on members review the correlational evidence about the of an American Indian tribe.69 For children differences in the mechanism by SES or the who’d never been poor, an increase in correlational links between each mechanism parental income had no effect on high school and parenting behavior. graduation or educational attainment. But Money to Spend on Children for children in poor families, the additional parental income increased schooling by If a main reason that low- and high-SES nearly one year and increased the chance of parents raise children differently is that graduating from high school by 30 percent. low-SES parents have less money, the policy More importantly, the windfall was associated solution would be to provide conditional or with a 5 percent increase in mothers’ and unconditional cash transfers to low-income fathers’ supervision of children and a 4 parents. It’s difficult to estimate the causal percent increase in positive mother-child effect of this on parenting behavior because interactions, according to children. These income increases are seldom random. But findings indicate that income itself can research on how low-income parents spend enhance parenting behavior and, as a result, their Earned Income Tax Credit (EITC) child outcomes. But note that the income refunds offers some insight into behavior increase in the study was significant—as changes caused by giving parents money. much as 100 percent for poor families, far The EITC is a tax credit that serves to offset more than that likely to be implemented by payroll taxes and supplement the wages of US public policies. So it’s unclear whether low-income workers. Studies that compare lessons from the study could guide real-world spending patterns across the year for EITC applications. VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 41
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan A new intervention might clarify the workers at large firms about scheduling potential impact on parenting behavior practices and wellbeing, and thus providing of smaller but still meaningful income much new evidence.72 increases. A group of prominent researchers recently launched Baby’s First Years, an Information, Values, and Preferences experimental program that randomly assigns Another possible strategy involves designing either an unconditional cash transfer of interventions to affect parents’ information $4,000 per year or a nominal $20 per month about, values around, or preferences for to low-income families. Over the next few investments in children. Impacting these years, the study will explore the effects mechanisms is one of the goals of home of these income boosts not only on infant visiting programs, which are by far the most development but also on parents’ mental common policy approach to narrowing health, parenting stress, and parenting parenting gaps between higher- and lower- practices. Baby’s First Years seeks to answer SES parents. These programs typically two questions: Can extra money alone target the mother-infant relationship, aiming enhance parenting practices? And if so, does to enhance child development by modeling it do so by improving parents’ mental health or directly instructing parents about caring and reducing parenting stress?70 for infants, toddlers, and preschool-aged children. This approach assumes that Time to Spend with Children parents who know the importance of certain Another policy approach that might alter parenting behaviors, and learn how to parents’ behavior would be to increase the engage in them, will do so more often. The amount or nature of the time available to number of families served by home visiting low-income parents to spend with their programs proliferated with the passage of children. But education-based differences in the Maternal, Infant, and Early Childhood parental time investments persist even when Home Visiting Program (MIECHV), part of policies provide generous family supports the Affordable Care Act in 2010; MIECHV in attempts to equalize opportunity for has been authorized through fiscal year child development.71 Still, given that stress 2022.73 disrupts parents’ mental health, focus, and Three of the most commonly adopted (and attention, policies that improve the stability rigorously evaluated) home visiting models and predictability of low-income parents’ are the Nurse-Family Partnership, the Early work schedules could make a meaningful Head Start home visiting program, and difference in parenting behavior and Healthy Families America.74 Although each children’s development. Studies to assess of these programs has demonstrated positive the impact of such approaches are still in effects on some parenting outcomes, the their infancy, but one multicomponent effects vary across sites, even within the investigation—the Shift Project, conducted programs. For example, multiple evaluations jointly at the University of California, of Healthy Families America have Berkeley, and the University of California, demonstrated modest effects on mothers’ San Francisco—holds promise. The project ability to interact in sensitive or stimulating is using an innovative method of data ways with children, but only at some collection to survey thousands of retail sites.75 Another home visiting program, the 42 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes Home Instruction Program for Preschool parents less likely to participate in home Youngsters (HIPPY), which specifically visiting, it’s just too difficult to deliver the targeted parents’ reading and educational programs as intended.80 activities with preschool-aged children, showed robust effects on children’s school The second reason for home visiting’s performance in the first cohort, but not in modest benefits may be that the chief later ones.76 Even when programs (or sites of mechanism these programs use to enhance programs) were found to enhance parenting parenting practices—providing information practices, the effects were relatively modest and instruction—isn’t the mechanism that differentiates the behavior of lower- and and often faded over time.77 higher-income parents. Above, we reviewed The most recent evidence on home visiting research that suggests low- and high-income comes from a national evaluation of the four parents largely share the same parenting most commonly used MIECHV-funded goals and values, and agree that it’s important programs: the three described above, and to conduct enriching activities with their the Parents as Teachers program. Like the children. The best evidence indicates that evaluations of individual programs before low- and high-income parents differ primarily it, the Mother and Infant Home Visiting in the level of stress each must negotiate Program Evaluation, or MIHOPE, found while parenting, and in the impact of certain that home visiting programs can increase cognitive biases on parenting decisions. the quality of cognitive stimulation in the Next we discuss two less time-consuming home and reduce the frequency with which approaches to parenting programs, parents use harsh or aggressive disciplinary sometimes called light touch interventions. approaches. But as in the previous studies, For this reason, they may hold particular these effects were modest in size.78 In promise. fact, only about one-third of the parenting outcomes that MIHOPE examined showed Family and Environmental Stress effects that were statistically significant. The home visiting programs we’ve described Why does home visiting tend to yield so far all aim to alleviate parental stress and only modest benefits? One reason may be improve parental mental health, among other that such programs typically have trouble goals. Yet only Healthy Families America has recruiting and retaining families.79 Programs demonstrated impacts on mothers’ mental often recruit fewer than their target number health, and only at some sites.81 MIHOPE of families and then provide far fewer home did find that home visiting, when averaged visits than they expect to. On average, across model programs, was associated with families in MIHOPE participated for only statistically significant reductions in mothers’ eight months, even though some programs depressive symptoms, but the effects were were designed to last years, and 17 percent small.82 These small and inconsistent findings of program mothers didn’t receive a single likely stem from the broad focus of these home visit. Perhaps because of the hectic programs—they don’t exclusively target schedules and nonstandard hours faced by mothers’ mental health or stress reduction— many low-income families, and because the and their difficulty in recruiting and retaining cognitive biases described above may make families. VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 43
Ariel Kalil and Rebecca Ryan largely sustained at three and six years after Programs that focus on the program ended.84 improving parents’ mental The results from these programs suggest health using clear clinical that some interventions can improve the quality of parenting in low-SES families approaches hold the most by reducing parents’ emotional and promise for enhancing interpersonal stress and improving their mental health. But long-term home visiting parenting behavior. programs with diffuse goals aren’t likely to produce those effects consistently or But a number of smaller, more targeted at practically significant levels. Rather, programs have found that improving programs that focus on improving parents’ parents’ mental health or reducing stress mental health using clear clinical approaches results in improved parenting behavior. hold the most promise for enhancing Mothers and Babies is one example. This parenting behavior. six-week cognitive-behavioral intervention Cognitive Biases and Decision-Making has been shown to reduce depressive symptoms and prevent depressive episodes In the past few years, a new approach to among women before and after giving supporting parents has emerged that avoids birth; as a result, it increases the mothers’ the difficulties of recruitment and retention sensitivity with their infants.83 The program in home visiting programs. These light- has been successfully embedded in touch, behavioral interventions, usually traditional home visiting programs, offering designed with scale-up in mind, typically a way to enhance home visiting’s effects on target specific, discrete parenting behaviors parenting behavior. Family Foundations to get at the cognitive biases that may is another well-developed and rigorously prevent mothers and fathers from using evaluated intervention to improve parents’ certain parenting practices. One example is mental health. This program aims to the Parents and Children Together (PACT) minimize the strains of the transition to Study, a field experiment conducted at the parenthood, and to help parents support Behavioral Insights and Parenting Lab at and not undermine each other. At six the University of Chicago, which tested a months and one year after a child’s birth, behavioral intervention to increase parent- mothers and fathers in Family Foundations child reading time among low-income reported significantly fewer depressive families.85 symptoms compared to a control group. Mothers also reported less anxiety, and both The PACT study hypothesized that present mothers and fathers reported more support bias might be the key to understanding why from each other. In interactions with their low-SES parents read aloud to their children children, parents showed more sensitivity less often than higher-SES parents do. The and more support for child exploration, intervention aimed to overcome this bias and had a more positive affect; intervention with a set of behavioral tools (goal-setting, fathers showed less negativity. Unlike those feedback, timely reminders, and social of most other programs, these effects were rewards) designed to “bring the future 44 T H E F U T UR E OF C HI L DRE N
Parenting Practices and Socioeconomic Gaps in Childhood Outcomes to the present” and help parents form children’s preschool absences. Based the habit of regular reading. These tools on outcome data from the preschools’ were all deployed using text messages, administrative records, the intervention rather than in-person staff visits, which decreased chronic absenteeism by 20 made the program relatively easy for low- percent over 18 weeks. The study’s income parents with hectic, unpredictable survey data showed that the parents who schedules and high levels of daily stress. benefited most were those who, at the start of the program, had reported lower On average, PACT more than doubled preferences for attendance. In short, the amount of time parents spent reading parents with weaker beliefs about the to their children (the study measured usefulness of preschool benefited the time use objectively, using digital tools). most from messages and reminders that But even more important was the finding emphasized its importance. that the intervention was substantially more effective for parents who were more Finally, to address challenges arising from present biased.86 In short, parents who cognitive scarcity, some promising new suffered from present bias were the very approaches focus on parents’ executive ones who benefited from an intervention function skills, key components of designed to overcome it. which include impulse control, working memory, and mental flexibility. These The PACT study doesn’t just suggest interventions seek to relieve the effects of that difficulty making temporal tradeoffs chronic toxic stress that can compromise is partly responsible for parents’ failure decision-making among low-income to read to their children. It also offers parents. Although experimental evidence a blueprint for managing this cognitive is currently lacking, some programs for bias. Using a set of known behavioral low-income parents are using coaching, tools, parents are able to increase desired multimedia, and specially designed behaviors and improve their decision- computer games to help adults improve making. Moreover, PACT’s cost per family memory, focus and attention, impulse was relatively low—a fraction of the per control, organization, problem-solving, and capita costs of current policy interventions multitasking.88 Mindfulness meditation designed to improve preschool children’s training, mind-body exercises (such as educational outcomes. This suggests that relaxation breathing), and “brain games” behaviorally based interventions can are other tools that may increase the feasibly be adapted for policy purposes. quality of parent-child interaction by In another example of this new approach, improving parents’ executive function Kalil and colleagues designed a behavioral skills—and likely improve mental health field experiment, Show Up to Grow Up, to and health outcomes as well.89 Like the increase attendance and diminish chronic other behavioral interventions described absences at subsidized preschool programs above, these programs could be deployed in Chicago.87 The program sent parents through technology in a way that could personalized text messages targeting make home visits or meetings at children’s behavioral bottlenecks that were driving preschools unnecessary. VOL. 30 / NO. 1 / S PR ING 2020 45
You can also read