Natural History of Trisomy 18 and Trisomy 13: 11. Psychomotor Development
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
American J o u r n a l of Medical Genetics 49:189-194 (1994) Natural History of Trisomy 18 and Trisomy 13: 11. Psychomotor Development Bonnie J. Baty, Lynn B. Jorde, Brent L. Blackburn, and John C. Carey Division of Medical Genetics, Department of Pediatrics (B.J.B., B.L.B., J.C.C.), and Department of Human Genetics (LB.J.j, University of Utah School of Medicine, Salt Lake City, Utah Developmental data were abstracted from KEY WORDS: trisomy 18, trisomy 13, chro- medical records on 50 trisomy 18 individuals mosome 18, chromosome 13, ranging in age from 1 to 232 months and 12 development trisomy 13 individuals ranging in age from 1 to 130 months. Data on the age when trisomy 18 and trisomy 13 children achieved develop- INTRODUCTION mental skills were collected from a larger group of 62 trisomy 18 individuals and 14 tri- Little has been written regarding the developmental somy 13 individuals whose families filled out assessment oftrisomy 18 and trisomy 13. Previous infor- parent questionnaires. Developmental quo- mation comes mainly from individual case reports of tient (DQ), defined as developmental age di- older individuals. A few of these case reports include vided by chronological age, averaged 0.18 for some detailed information about developmental pro- trisomy 18 and 0.25 for trisomy 13. There was a gress [Gerhard, 1976; Smith et al., 1978; Smith et al., dramatic drop in DQ from infancy to later 19891but most give sketchy information, if any. This is childhood. The highest DQs and the greatest in large part due to the rarity of older individuals with variation in DQs were in the first 2-3 years of full trisomies and the fact that any one practitioner is life. Developmental ages in 7 skill areas were unlikely to see several older trisomy 18 or 13 children. significantly different, with daily living and The parent support group offered an opportunity to receptive language having the highest values study a series of surviving individuals. One recent pub- and motor and communication skills having lication has reported on a series of older individuals, the lowest. When chronological age was taken using the S.O.F.T. support group for identification of into account, there was no significant differ- cases [Van Dyck and Allen, 19901. ence in DQs in the same 7 skill areas, although there was a trend that was similar to the pat- METHODS tern of differences with developmental age. This study is part of a larger study of the natural Older children could use a walker, under- history of trisomy 18 and trisomy 13. The initial part of stand words and phrases, use a few words the study involved sending questionnaires to families and/or signs, crawl, follow simple commands, registered with the Support Organization for Trisomy recognize and interact with others, and play 18, 13, and Related Disorders, which is known as independently. Walking and some toileting S.O.F.T. A total of 98 trisomy 18families and 32 trisomy skills were also reported for trisomy 13. Al- 13 families with nonmosaic trisomy answered a 4-page though individuals with trisomy 18 and tri- questionnaire, which included questions about demo- somy 13 were clearly functioning in the severe graphics, birth data, growth, neonatal hospitalization(s) to profound developmentally handicapped and operations, immunizations, psychomotor develop- range, they did achieve some psychomotor ment, birth defects, medical complications, cause of maturation and always continued to learn. death, and family history of other chromosome disor- 0 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc. ders. Data concerning all aspects except development have been reported in a separate publication [Baty et al., 19941.Cytogenetjc confirmation was achieved for 97%of trisomy 18 individuals and 97% of trisomy 13 individ- uals. Ages at the time of the developmental assessment Received for publication March 5,1993; revision received August ranged from 1 to 232 months (19 years, 4 months) for 16,1993. trisomy 18 and 1t o 130 months (10years, 10months) for Address reprint requests to Bonnie Jeanne Baty, Department of trisomy 13. There were 41 trisomy 18 individuals (42%) Pediatrics, University of Utah Medical Center, Salt Lake City, UT and 12 trisomy 13 individuals (38%)surviving to age 1 84112. year. The developmental questions consisted of a list of 0 1994 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
190 Baty et al. developmental stages, and parents were asked the age or RESULTS date at which their child had achieved those stages. A total of 62 trisomy 18 families and 14 trisomy 13 fami- The psychomotor development reported by parents is lies provided information on psychomotor development. summarized in Table I. The table contains the mean, The second part of the study involved obtaining medi- standard error of the mean, range, number of individ- cal records from the same families that filled out the uals reporting each stage, and the normal age range parent questionnaires. The two groups did not com- [Frankenburg et al., 1976; Moyers et al., 19741 for each pletely overlap, because we could not obtain medical skill. records on some individuals whose parents supplied de- As is often the case with conditions associated with velopmental information, and we obtained medical re- mental retardation, the range of time in which a devel- cords on some individuals whose parents did not supply opmental level is achieved is wider than usual. Four of developmental information. The subset of individuals the trisomy 18 children and 1 trisomy 13 child were with developmental records all had confirmation of full reported to use a few consistent signs. Five trisomy 18 trisomy. If developmental testing had ever been done, children used a few consistent words. Many of the older records on the child's psychomotor development were children have a much more extensive receptive vocabul- requested. We obtained developmental records on 50 ary. Five of the trisomy 18 children walked with a individuals with trisomy 18 and 12 individuals with walker, and one trisomy 18 child and 2 trisomy 13 chil- trisomy 13. Of the individuals with developmental re- dren cruised around furniture. No individual with tri- cords, 29 with trisomy 18 and 8 with trisomy 13 were somy 18 was reported to walk, but one trisomy 13 child over the age of 1 year. walked at age 9. In addition to the child with trisomy 13 Data were abstracted from the developmental records reported, two of the authors have personally met an- and computerized using the Ingres database manage- other 13 year old with trisomy 13 who walks. ment program. Statistical analysis was performed using The remainder of the data reported was abstracted by the BMDP package. The information abstracted in- one of the authors (BJB)from the medical records. Table cluded chronological ages, developmental ages for spe- I1 shows the mean CA, mean DA, and mean DQ for cific skill areas on specific developmental tests, individ- various age groups, along with the standard error of the ual skills recorded by the developmental specialists mean for each. The number of observations in each evaluating the child, feeding skills, and IQs. Many chil- group and the number of children on which these obser- dren were evaluated at multiple ages using multiple vations were made are also reported. Figure 1 summa- tests, while some children had very few entries. There rizes the data on DQ in graphic form. For both condi- were 28 different developmental tests used. The most tions, mean DQs drop dramatically with age (one-way common (the Bayley Scales of Infant Development, the analysis of variance; P
Natural History of Trisomy 18 and 13: 11 191 TABLE 11. Mean ( t Standard Error) Chronological Ages, Developmental Ages, and Developmental Quotients for Trisomy 18 and Trisomy 13 Chron age Dev age Number of Number of Age (years) Trisomy (months) (months) DQ observations children 0-1 18 8.6 (20.6) 3.7 (k0.5) 0.42 ( t 0.04) 25 10 13 6.6 ( t 0.7) 3.3 (t 0. 5) 0.48 ( k 0.06) 11 3 1-3 18 25.2 ( t 0.8) 3.9 (t0.2) 0.17 (&0.01) 76 15 13 26.3 (kO.9) 7.7 (20.4) 0.31 ( k0.02) 53 5 3-5 18 46.8 ( k 1.2) 8.2 (t0.8) 0.18 (t0.02) 48 7 13 44.1 (irl.0) 7.3 ( k 0 . 5 ) 0.17 (tO.01) 26 5 >5 18 97.6 (24.4) 6.8 ( k 0.4) 0.08 (+0.01) 58 9 13 92.7 ( t 6 . 1 ) 13.3 ( k 1.9) 0.13 (20.01) 25 2 All ages 18 48.5 ( 2 2.6) 5.7 (&0.3) 0.18 (kO.01) 207 26 13 42.9 (k3.0) 8.4 ( k 0.5) 0.25 (20.01) 115 8 data. The highest individual DQs, as well as the great- ideally be a horizontal line at 1.0. The drop-off in the est variation in DQs, clearly occur at the youngest ages. curve does not represent a loss of skills, but rather There are many individual measurements above 0.5. greater distance from the normal curve. It is clear from These higher DQs represent 6 different cases (4have the information derived from parent reports and from trisomy 18 and 2 have trisomy 13).Three of the children these formal developmental assessments that these chil- have only one measurement above 0.5, and 3 children dren generally continue to acquire new skills through- have from 3 to 6 measurements above 0.5. The CAs of out their lives. these high DQs range from 2 months t o 50 months. They An average DQ a t each CA for each child was also represent several different tests and skill areas. Thus, plotted to determine whether multiple measurements there seems to be no pattern of high DQ measurements, on the same child at the same age could bias the data. All except that they are all at 50 months or below. The three plots (for trisomy 18, trisomy 13 and combined measurements above 100 months represent 1trisomy 13 data) looked essentially the same. Once again, trisomy child and 3 trisomy 18 children. The values for the tri- 13 appeared to have somewhat higher scores in the older somy 13 child are higher than those of the trisomy 18 children. children, but the numbers are too small to permit gener- Only two children had IQ scores recorded in their alization. In general, there is a sharp “drop-off’in DQ, developmental records, both trisomy 18children with IQ representing an increasing distance from the develop- scores of 20. The children were ages 5 and 12. We did not mental curve of average normal children, which would find IQ scores useful, both because ofthe lack ofdata and Developmental Quotient 0.5 0.4- 0.3 - ’ 0.2 - 0.1 - ’ < 12 12-36 36-60 Age in Months MTrisomy 18 HTrisomy 13 Fig. 1. Average developmental quotients a t different ages in trisomy 18 and trisomy 13.
192 Batyet al. a Developmental Quotient 0.8 .... 0.6 - .. 0.4 .. . ... .. . . ' 0.2 , , , c .. 0 I I . , I 0 50 100 150 200 Age in Months Developmental Quotient I[ 0.61- . . 0.41 .. '.. . . . ' I I 1 1. , 1 I 0 20 40 60 80 100 120 140 Age in Months Fig. 2. (a)Developmental quotients (individual developmental age measurements divided by chrono- logical age) by age in trisomy 18. (b) Developmental quotients (individual developmental age measure- ments divided by chronological age) by age in trisomy 13. the relative lack of descriptive power of a single I& score showed that these 3 tests gave significantly different in the lower end of the IQ range. mean values of both CA (P
Natural History of Trisomy 18 and 13: I1 193 TABLE 111. Developmental Assessment the activities commonly documented in the first year by Three Common Developmental Tests* were following, cooing, rolling, smiling responsively, Mean ( & SE) Mean ( 2 SE) Mean ( 2 SE) reaching, and recognizing close adults. In the next 2 CA (months) DA (months) DQ (months) years, new activities included sitting supported, object 5.0 (f0.5) 0.19 (fO.02) permanence, imitation, playing baby games, sitting in- Bayley 40.0 (26.3) Alpern 59.7 ( k 8.0) 6.3 ( ? 0.7) 0.22 ( f0.05) dependently, and recognizing words. In the next 3 years Vineland 75.3 (k8.1) 6.9 (20.5) 0.13 ( 2 0 . 0 2 ) (at ages 4-6) commando crawling, independent playing, * Some tests are combined. For example, a Bayley test may be given following simple commands, helping with hygiene alone or in combination with any other developmental test, and it will tasks, standing, understanding cause and effect, and be counted as a n entry. use of signs were reported. The older children could identify common objects, use a walker, crawl, and under- stand words and phrases. Trisomy 13 individuals at- The data were divided into 7 main skill areas: recep- tained the same skills, and some also acquired some tive language, expressive language, communication, toileting skills and walking ability. The data on specific daily living, cognitive, social, and motor skills. There developmental skills which were collected from parents were no significant differences in mean CAs between the of these same children were very consistent with the 7 skill areas. There were also no significant differences data obtained from formal developmental evaluations. in CA between trisomy 18 and trisomy 13. We also abstracted information about feeding skills There were highly significant differences in DAs be- from the medical records. For trisomy 18, 33/50 (66%) tween many skill areas, using one-way analysis of vari- documented gavage feeding, 54% as newborns. Breast ance (P
194 Baty e t al. TABLE V. Mean Developmental Quotient ( r Standard Error) by Skill Area All Trisomy 18 Trisomy 13 Rec language 0.24 (20.04) Rec language 0.20 ( f0.05) Cognitive 0.33 ( ? 0.06) Daily living 0.23 (h0.04) Cognitive 0.19 (kO.03) Rec lang 0.31 ( t 0.08) Cognitive 0.22 (‘0.03) Daily living 0.19 (fO.05) Daily living 0.26 ( z 0.06) Social * 0.21 ( 0.02) Social 0.18 (r0.03) Social 0.26 (t0.04) Expr language 0.19 ( 2 0.03) Communication 0.15 (20.03) Expr language 0.23 ( 2 0.06) Communication 0.18 (k0.03) Expr language 0.14 (-c0.03) Communication 0.23 (k0.05) Motor 0.18 ( -t 0.01) Motor 0.14 (20.01) Motor 0.23 i? 0.021 data have a negative exponential distribution and accomplishments of their children are acknowledged by are similar in both trisomy 18 and 13, although tri- the medical community. The range of DQ variation for somy 13 had significantly higher DQs. The highest different developmental skill areas is relatively small DQs and the greatest variation in DQs were in the and may even seem inconsequential. However, a differ- first 2-3 years of life. The drop in DQ does not re- ence of several months of developmental skills in the flect a loss of skills, but instead reflects an increased first year of life has great meaning to families. It could lag in developmental progress compared to normal mean the difference between a child who sits alone vs. a children. child who cannot sit unsupported, or the difference be- 2. Developmental ages in 7 skill areas were signif- tween nonresponsiveness vs. smiling, reaching out and icantly different, with daily living and receptive lan- recognizing close adults. It is important that families guage having the highest values and motor and have this information when a diagnosis of trisomy 18 or communication skills having the lowest. When chro- trisomy 13 is made, either prenatally or postnatally. nological age was taken into account, there was no significant difference in DQs in the same 7 skill ACKNOWLEDGMENTS areas, although there was a trend that was similar Dr. Jorde was supported by NIH Grant HG-00347 and to the pattern of differences with developmental age. NSF Grant BNS-8720330. We would like to thank the 3. Although individuals with trisomy 18 and 13 many families who provided information about their were clearly functioning in the severe to profound de- child’s development and/or developmental records. velopmentally handicapped range, they did achieve many skills of childhood, and always continued to REFERENCES learn. Older children could use a walker, understand Baty BJ, Blackburn BL, Carey J (1994):The natural history of trisomy words and phrases, use a few words andlor signs, 18 and trisomy 13: I. Growth, physical assessment, medical histo- crawl, follow simple commands, recognize and inter- ries, survival and recurrence risk. Am J Med Genet 49:175-188. act with others, and play independently. Walking Bos AP, Broers CJM, Hazebroek FWJ, van Hemel JO, Tjbboel D, and some toileting skills were also reported for tri- Wesby-van Swaay E, Molenaar JC (1992):Avoidance of emergency surgery in newborn infants with trisomy 18. Lancet 339:913-915. somy 13. Frankenburg WK, van Doorninck WJ, Liddell TN, Dick NP (1976):The Denver Rescreening Developmental Questionnaire (PDQ). Pedi- In the course of our contact with these families, it atrics 57344-753. became clear that many parents resented the early mes- Gerhard M (1976): Development of motor skills in a child with tri- sage that their child would never interact with his or her somy-18. Dev Med Child Neurol 18:538. environment and family. Many professionals conclude Moyers R, Hartsook J, Kopel H (1974):Facial growth and dentition. In that a diagnosis of trisomy 18 or trisomy 13 means a Lowey GH (ed): “Growth and Development of Children,” 6th ed. Chicago: Year Book Medical Publishers, pp 354-384. hopeless outlook with survival in a vegetative state, or Smith A, Silink M, Ruxton T (1978):Trisomy 18 in a n 11year old girl. that the diagnosis is incompatible with life [Bos, 19921. J Ment Defic Res 22:277-286. Many families with surviving trisomy 18 or trisomy 13 Smith A, Field B, Learoyd BM (1989):Trisomy 18at age 21 years. Am J children think that the information they were given was Med Genet 34:338-339. more discouraging than necessary, and ignored the hu- Van Dyke DC, Allen M (1990):Clinical management considerations in manity of their child. It is important to parents that the long-term survivors with trisomy 18. Pediatrics 85753-759.
You can also read