Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite Soccer Performance

Page created by Anne Collins
 
CONTINUE READING
Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite Soccer Performance
Department of Business Administration

            UZH Business Working Paper Series

                     Working Paper No. 391

 Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite
                       Soccer Performance

 Carlos Gomez-Gonzalez, Helmut Dietl, David Berri, and Cornel
                              Nesseler

                   First Draft: 01 November 2021
                    Current Draft: 20 May 2022

       University of Zurich, Plattenstrasse 14, CH-8053 Zurich,
          http://www.business.uzh.ch/forschung/wps.html
Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite Soccer Performance
UZH Business Working Paper Series

               Contact Details

          Carlos Gomez Gonzalez

             University of Zurich

   Department of Business Administration

Plattenstrasse 14, CH-8032 Zurich, Switzerland

       carlos.gomez@business.uzh.ch

            Tel.: +41 44 634 53 14
Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite Soccer Performance
Gender Bias in Perceived Quality. An Experiment with Elite Soccer Performance

Abstract

Whether one looks at revenue, investment, or coverage, men’s sports do better than women’s.

Many assume that the differences are driven by absolute differences in quality of athletic

performance. However, the existence of stereotypes should alert us to another possibility: What if

perceived quality is filtered through gender stereotypes? We perform an experiment showing

participants video clips of elite female and male soccer players. In the control group, participants

evaluated normal videos where the gender of the players was clear to see. In the treatment group,

participants evaluated the same videos but with gender obscured by blurring. We find that

participants only rated men’s videos higher when they knew they were watching men. When they

didn’t know who they were watching, ratings for female and male athletes did not differ

significantly. The findings are consistent with the interpretation that gender bias plays a role in

the evaluation of athletic performance. Implications for research and the sports industry are

discussed.

Keywords: experiment; evaluation; gender bias; fans; soccer; women’s sport

                                                  1
1. Introduction

Stereotypical judgments and misperceptions influence the slow professional advancement of

women (Ellemers, 2018; Valian, 1999). Research shows that negative gender stereotypes about

women’s performance exist in business (Gupta & Turban, 2012; Heilman, 2001), education

(Boring, 2017; Mengel et al., 2019), politics (Dolan & Sanbonmatsu, 2009; Lawless, 2009),

science (Moss-Racusin et al., 2012; Régner et al., 2019; Witteman et al., 2019), and sports

(Burton, 2015; Gomez-Gonzalez et al., 2019). In male-dominated occupations, women routinely

need to deal with criticism of their talent and hardiness (Leslie et al., 2015; Morganson et al.,

2010).

         In sports, the abilities of female athletes are constantly questioned by some fans and

reporters on TV, Twitter, Facebook, and other media outlets (Hyde, 2019; Scheadler & Wagstaff,

2018; Trolan, 2013). Investment and media coverage differences are evident and striking as

women’s sports continue to get very little attention (Cooky et al., 2013; Kassouf, 2020;

Matheson, 2021; UNESCO, 2021). Men’s professional sports leagues outperform their women’s

counterparts in economic terms, including demand figures, broadcasting revenue, franchise value,

and athlete pay (Zimbalist, 2019).1

         From a managerial perspective, the question is why this difference exists and what can be

done. Literature on objective and perceived quality offers helpful insights into this question

(Mitra & Golder, 2006). While objective quality of a product is based on measurable attributes,

1
  For example, in December 2019 it was announced that David Tepper had paid $325 million for a new Major
League Soccer (MLS) franchise in Charlotte, North Carolina (Negley, 2019). Just a few months later, it was thought
the expansion fee for a new National Women's Soccer League (NWSL) team in Sacramento, California, was between
$1 and $2 million (Kassouf, 2020). That same year, the Seattle Reign—an existing NWSL team—sold to French
investors for less than $4 million. These disparities in private investment are also seen with respect to public
investment (Matheson, 2021).

                                                        2
perceived quality is based on subjective judgments. It certainly is the case that women’s and

men’s soccer are different and that, for example, female soccer players run slower than male

players (Cheuvront et al., 2005). However, it is debatable that physical differences are what

drives demand. The objective quality of women’s and men’s games is difficult to measure, and

impossible for managers to influence.

       Perceived quality, however, has considerable managerial implications. Managers can

influence perception, and perception influences demand (García & Rodríguez, 2002; Pawlowski

et al., 2018). Stakeholders change the perception of quality among sports fans through, for

example, coverage, promotion, and engagement strategies. We argue that those watching men’s

and women’s soccer today have a gender bias that influences the perception of quality. Men’s

soccer appears more often on TV and players get paid more (Cooky et al., 2013; Zimbalist,

2019); therefore, fans tend to believe that the men’s version is better.

       We performed an experiment to analyze the influence that gender stereotypes may have

on the perceived quality of women’s and men’s soccer. Participants were shown videos of

professional women’s and men’s soccer players scoring goals. In the control group, videos were

unmodified. In the treatment group, the gender of the players was blurred, making it impossible

for a viewer to know if they were watching men or women. Participants were then asked to

evaluate the overall performance of the players. The results show that when participants could

identify the gender of the players, male players were rated higher. When the gender of the players

could not be ascertained, however, no significant differences between ratings of female and male

athletes were detected. The results have implications for the coverage of women’s sports in the

media and how the performance of female athletes is perceived (Fink, 2015).

                                                  3
The approach adopted here has been used in other research settings. For example, in the

hiring process, research shows that employers tend to discriminate less when the gender of the

candidate is hidden (Åslund & Skans, 2012; Goldin & Rouse, 2000; Krause et al., 2012). The

concept of blinding identity features to improve decisions has also been explored in science

(Budden et al., 2008; Johnson & Kirk, 2020), education (Hinnerich et al., 2015), and criminal

justice (Sah et al., 2015). To our knowledge, ours is the first study to empirically analyze the

effect of blinded decision-making in the context of gender bias and professional sports.

       The paper is structured as follows. Section 2 reviews the literature and presents the

theoretical framework on hegemonic masculinity and gender imprinting. Section 3 describes the

experimental design and method; Section 4 the results. Section 5 discusses implications and

limitations, and concludes the paper.

2. Theoretical framework and literature review

To date, research on women’s sports has developed some key theoretical questions (Cunningham,

2013; Fink, 2013). Specific topics have been studied such as the underrepresentation of women in

leadership roles (Burton, 2015; Darvin et al., 2021; Gomez-Gonzalez et al., 2019), group

diversity and team and organizational outcomes (Lee & Cunningham, 2018; Taylor et al., 2022;

Wicker et al., 2019), and demand and consumption (Funk et al., 2003; Valenti et al., 2020).

Thomson et al. (2022) argue advancing women’s professional sports further may benefit from

multidisciplinary research.

       Women’s sports leagues are experiencing remarkable growth in attention and

consumption, supporting athletes competing at the highest level (Litchfield & Kavanagh, 2019;

Pegoraro et al., 2018; Taylor et al., 2020). In 2020, in the US, the National Women’s Soccer

League broke its viewership records by nearly 300% (Gao, 2020). The NCAA Women’s

                                                  4
Gymnastics Championship and the Women’s National Basketball Association are other North

American leagues with record-breaking viewership figures. In Europe, the trend is similar. FC

Barcelona Femení has recently registered world-record attendance figures in soccer, with crowds

of over 90,000 spectators (Burhan, 2022).

       Despite this growth and its business potential, women’s sports have no shortage of

detractors (Hyde, 2019; Scheadler & Wagstaff, 2018; Trolan, 2013). Negative comments target

women’s physical attributes (Burch et al., 2018), sexuality (Cavalier & Newhall, 2018), athletic

skills (Joncheray et al., 2016), and overall value (Micelotta et al., 2018). Even female fans are not

immune to detrimental perceptions (Sveinson et al., 2019). This negative notion about women’s

sports may have an influence on the perception of quality. The negativity surround women’s

sports should, we argue, be understood within a context of hegemonic masculinity (Hindman &

Walker, 2020; Hoeber & Kerwin, 2013) and gender imprinting (Gisladottir & Reid, 2021;

Micelotta et al., 2018).

2.1. Hegemonic masculinity

Hegemonic masculinity refers to practices and beliefs that establish and reinforce men’s control

and status over other groups, including women (Connell & Messerschmidt, 2005). Scholars argue

that stereotypical attributes of men (e.g., strength, competitiveness, and risk preferences) are

overemphasized in sports (Englis, 2017). The lack of these attributes is often cited in social media

critiques of women’s athletics (Hyde, 2019) and as justifications for the underrepresentation of

women in sports leadership positions (Burton, 2015).

       In sports management research, scholars have discussed the relationship between

hegemonic masculinity and the role of women in sports at the organizational level (Burton, 2015;

                                                  5
Hoeber & Kerwin, 2013). For example, Hindman and Walker (2020) argue that gender

stereotypes result in disadvantages for women in hiring, promotion, and evaluation.

       Such gender stereotypes require women to deal with criticisms of their talent and strength,

and lead people to believe that men are a better fit for the job. Men are often perceived as more

competent and evaluated better than women with equal qualifications for sport-related positions

(Burton et al., 2012). However, the gender stereotypes that reward masculinity traits go beyond

the organizational culture of specific institutions (LaVoi & Dutove, 2012). They have the power

to influence the public’s perception of women’s performance.

2.2. Gender imprinting

Micelotta et al. (2018) introduced the concept of “gender imprinting” in the context of sports.

Broadly, this concept refers to embedded sociocultural gender attributes that define values,

norms, and beliefs in specific industries. The gender imprinting of the sports industry is rooted in

its origin, when hegemonic masculinity shaped organizational norms, defined the sports culture,

and desired practices (Gisladottir & Reid, 2021; Micelotta et al., 2018). Therefore, gender

imprinting is closely related to the global level of hegemonic masculinity (Connell &

Messerschmidt, 2005).

       Although some societies and industries are taking steps to advance gender equity

(Mendoza & Zurka, 2021; United Nations, 2022), gender stereotypes and prejudices are likely to

influence behavior today. For example, Micelotta et al. (2018) show that cultural barriers

negatively affect the capacity of new ventures to acquire symbolic and material support in

women’s sports. The sociocultural level of women’s sports is imprinted with prejudices about

athletes’ abilities, which determine the capacity to compete with the dominant male sports culture

(Scheadler & Wagstaff, 2018).

                                                 6
Constant exposure to negative comments about women’s attributes, athletic skills, and

overall value—imprinted within the industry and embedded in society—can negatively influence

the views and perceptions of fans (Travers, 2008). We argue that gender stereotypes negatively

influence perceived quality among fans who watch women’s games, highlights, or any other

sport-related content. We hypothesize that most fans would not perceive any differences in

quality between top male and female players—if the gender was not clear to see.

H1. Fans evaluate women’s performance lower only if the gender of the players is clear to see.

3. Method

3.1. Experimental videos

An experiment tested this hypothesis. We selected 10 videos from women’s and men’s club and

national team matches in 2019. The videos were between 5 and 14 seconds each. We used goal-

scoring plays that were chosen by either UEFA or FIFA as highlights for the season. Sports

highlights are an increasing touchpoint to connect with the younger generations, who prefer

short-form content to consume and share on social media platforms (Silverman, 2020). All videos

featured highly skilled players and contained some of the most-watched goals on TV and social

media. Appendix Table A1 provides information about the match, scoring player, and length of

the videos.

       To conceal the gender of the players, we manipulated every video frame. Appendix 2

shows the parameters of the manipulated videos in Adobe Premiere Pro. The same mask feather,

opacity, and expansion levels were used for each frame; then the number of blocks (for

blurriness) were selected. The lowest level of blurriness that still allowed us to effectively

conceal the gender of the players was used. We blurred all human-related parts (players and

fans). Although this approach creates noise of its own for the evaluation of the play, pretests

                                                  7
showed that blurring only critical areas such as players’ heads was not enough to effectively

conceal gender. Figure 1 shows an example of the two versions of the videos, original and

blurred.

                                           [Figure 1 near here]

        Two groups were created. In the control group, 290 participants evaluated a sequence of

10 videos in which nothing was blurred (similar to Figure 1, left panel) In the treatment group,

323 participants evaluated the same sequence but with blurred videos (similar to Figure 1, right

panel). Thus, the only difference between groups was the blurriness of players and fans. Each

sequence consisted of five women’s and five men’s soccer videos, presented in random order (see

Appendix Table A2).

3.2. Participants and survey structure

The Amazon Mechanical Turk (AMT) was used to recruit participants. AMT is an online

marketplace in which workers exchange their labor for a monetary reward. AMT results in

heterogeneous samples (e.g., in nationality and age). Outcomes do not significantly differ from

traditional methods (Arechar et al., 2018; Snowberg & Yariv, 2021) and are not affected by

monetary incentives (Snowberg & Yariv, 2021). Employers decide payment and may specify

additional worker qualifications.

        In our experiment, the task was to complete a survey estimated to take 5 minutes. There

were no qualification requirements; the pay was $1.00. Relative to most AMT tasks, the

workload was low and the payment high. A sample of 613 participants was recruited (276

women, 337 men; mean age 34 years). The geographic breakdown was Africa (4), Asia (10),

Europe (225), North America (322), Oceania (2), and South America (39). Eleven participants

did not report their nationality.

                                                 8
Each participant was given a task labeled “Answer a survey about soccer” with the

description “Evaluate soccer performance (about 5 minutes).” We added the keywords survey;

sport; soccer; video. Participants answered questions in survey screens. Before evaluating the

videos, each participant was asked to provide information regarding age, gender, and country.

Participants were also asked to answer the following questions with binary response possibilities:

“Do you watch soccer?”; “Do you watch soccer daily (highlights or matches)?”; and “Do you

prefer men’s or women’s soccer?”

        These questions were chosen because people who watch soccer regularly could show

different behavior when evaluating women’s and men’s soccer. Additionally, research suggests

that behavior may be moderately influenced by explicit beliefs and values (Crandall & Eshleman,

2003; Devine et al., 2017). Therefore, we may find a relationship between the preference for

women’s or men’s soccer and the evaluation of the groups, especially when the videos are

unmodified and the gender visible.

        In the subsequent screens, after every video, we asked participants (1) to rate the

performance of the players on a 5-point scale: “Overall, the performance of the players is” (1 =

poor; 5 = excellent) and (2) to indicate whether they would pay to watch the match: “Would you

pay to watch the match?” (yes/no). The videos and questions were shown on subsequent survey

screens, and participants had to submit the final task on the last screen. Participants could take up

to 20 minutes to complete the survey and go back and forth among the screens.2

4. Results

Considering first the individual videos, 9 of 10 received a lower rating when they were blurred

(see Figure 2). Three unblurred men’s videos received an average evaluation > 4; none of the

2
 In a small subsample, we found no differences when the demographic questions were asked after evaluating the
videos rather than before.

                                                      9
unblurred women’s videos did so. As expected, blurred videos received lower evaluations (M =

3.52) than the blurred ones (M = 4.22). We expected this result as the blurred videos can create

some noise in perceived quality. Whether blurred or unblurred, the video evaluations show

similar trends. The key point for our research, however, is how participants evaluate men’s and

women’s soccer videos within the two different experimental groups.

                                           [Figure 2 near here]

       As shown in Figure 3, when the videos were not blurred and hence the gender visible,

participants rated men’s performance significantly higher than women’s (men’s videos, M =

4.012; women’s videos, M = 3.839; average treatment effect (ATE) = 0.173; Mann-Whitney U, z

= 3.429, p = 0.00, N = 580). However, when the videos were blurred and hence gender not

visible, participants’ ratings did not differ significantly between men’s (M = 3.583) and women’s

(M = 3.556) videos (ATE = 0.027; Mann-Whitney U, z = 0.846, p = 0.398, N = 646).

                                           [Figure 3 near here]

       To verify that the results are robust, we controlled for various covariates. Table 1 shows

the regression results with the ratings for unblurred videos as the dependent variable. We

included all control variables stepwise. Model 1 presents only the evaluation of women’s and

men’s videos. Model 2 includes participant demographics. Model 3 includes participants’

preference for women’s or men’s soccer (omitted responses are scored as no preference). Model

4 includes control variables for different sequences and participant country. Most control

variables had no statistically significant influence, although participants who watch soccer daily

made lower ratings overall. Nonetheless, the control variables throughout the models do not

influence the participants’ higher evaluation of men’s videos.

                                            [Table 1 near here]

                                                10
Table 2 shows the regression results with the ratings for blurred videos as the dependent

variable. Participants no longer rate men’s and women’s videos differently. This result is

consistent across all models that include the controls. Participants who prefer men’s soccer

evaluated the blurred videos significantly lower. Women evaluated the blurred videos

significantly higher than men. We lost 84 observations in Model 3 as we made a coding error

with the question regarding participants’ preference for male or female soccer. Omitting these

observations in Models 1 and 2 has no significant impact.

                                            [Table 2 near here]

       In Appendix Table A3, we tested the interaction between video gender and participant

gender to examine if women and men participants evaluate unblurred videos differently. Men

evaluated female videos significantly lower than male videos, but for women there was no

difference between men’s and women’s videos. Consistent with the results reported in Tables 1

and 2, although smaller in magnitude, when the players’ gender is visible (Table A4), fans report

to be significantly less likely to pay to watch a women’s match; when gender not visible (Table

A5), the effect disappears.

5. Discussion and Conclusions

Many factors contribute to gender disparities in professional advancement (Ellemers, 2018;

Valian, 1999). In many fields, women need to cope with stereotypes that question their ability to

succeed (Leslie et al., 2015; Morganson et al., 2010). Their representation and visibility are

severely limited. For example, in sports, the media seldom cover men and women equally (Cooky

et al., 2013). While 40% of all sports participants are women, women’s sports receive only

around 4% of the total coverage (UNESCO, 2021).

                                                 11
Some research suggests that preferences drive the type of content displayed by the media

(Gentzkow & Shapiro, 2006). Thus, fans prefer watching male sports because they derive a

higher utility. However, there is reason to suspect that fans are not evaluating men’s and

women’s sports by the same criteria. For example, fans of men’s sports seem quite comfortable

with evaluating male athletes relative to their competition. As we see in boxing, absolute

differences are often not even considered. When comparing men and women, though, absolute

differences are often cited.

       The results presented here support the hypothesis that gender stereotypes play a role in

perceived quality. Differences between men’s and women’s performance are perceived only

when the gender of the players is visible. When the gender of the players is no longer

distinguishable, men’s higher ratings disappear. Conceptually, the phenomenon may be summed

up by saying that gender biases “imprinted” in the sports context influence how fans perceive

women’s and men’s professional sports (Micelotta et al., 2018).

       This finding is relevant for players, managers, and broadcasters alike. Men’s soccer is

omnipresent throughout societies, through television and social media (Cooky et al., 2013;

Kassouf, 2020; Matheson, 2021; UNESCO, 2021). It is a common assumption that differences in

physiology between men’s and women’s soccer result in recognizable differences in quality.

However, our results show that this assumption must be reevaluated as gender bias may play an

important role. This bias in sports consumption is consistent with discourses of global hegemonic

masculinity (Burton, 2015; Connell & Messerschmidt, 2005; Hindman & Walker, 2020; Hoeber

& Kerwin, 2013) and gender imprinting (Gisladottir & Reid, 2021; Micelotta et al., 2018).

       Moderator factors regarding participants’ demographics, preferences, and habits influence

overall evaluation, but do not cancel out the gender bias. For example, participants who watch

                                                12
soccer daily rated both blurred and original videos lower than other participants; apparently, the

more you watch soccer, the more it takes to impress you. We did not find the expected influence

of explicit beliefs and values on behavior (Crandall & Eshleman, 2003; Devine et al., 2017).

Participants who express a preference for men’s or women’s soccer do not show a consistent bias

when evaluating the unblurred videos.

5.1. Limitations and Future Research

The findings from the experiment are robust but should be interpreted cautiously. This study has

limitations that suggest opportunities for future research. First, we selected videos that show only

elite performance, which could lead to a ceiling effect. The videos had already been filtered

through the choices of sports media broadcasters and pushed to the top of the list by people who

are not part of our study. Therefore, the focus on popular highlights limits the extent of the

implications. Other videos involving less skilled players could potentially prompt different

evaluations.

       Second, we used exclusively goal plays. Modern sports consumption trends show a

growing preference for brief highlights, especially among millennials and Gen Z (Silverman,

2020). However, a soccer match consists of various scenes that can also be interesting for viewers

(e.g., tackles, corners, free kicks, penalty kicks), and many enjoy watching whole matches. Third,

the experimental design includes only 10 videos because of the time needed to modify the videos

to conceal gender (2–6 weeks each). A larger number of videos would ensure higher variability

and allow us to draw more conclusions.

       Finally, we cannot rule out the possibility that other variables explain better the observed

relationship. For example, we did not control for the atmosphere in the stadium. Participants

watched the videos without any sound as commentators frequently reveal the gender of the

                                                 13
player. People often go to the stadium or watch a match not because of the quality but because of

the atmosphere. Future research can also explore other sports and exploit new experimental

designs. Artificial intelligence and machine learning algorithms have the potential to contribute to

this line of research.

5.2. Implications and Concluding Remarks

As societies advance in women’s rights, equity, and equality (Mendoza & Zurka, 2021; United

Nations, 2022), the exposure of future generations to gender stereotypes will decrease. In the

absence of a gender bias, our study show that the perceived quality of men’s and women’s elite

soccer may not differ. This result has managerial implications, as investments in women’s team

sports in terms of money and coverage should become highly attractive.

        Perceived quality is a subjective dimension that managers and leagues can deliberately

influence. This dimension is essential for sports consumption because how fans perceive quality

drives demand (García & Rodríguez, 2002; Pawlowski et al., 2018). The current trends in

women’s sports consumption should be sending signals to managers. The perceived quality of

women’s sports is already changing; fans are breaking attendance records and demanding more

coverage of women’s soccer (Burhan, 2022; Gao, 2020).

        In theory, online communication forms and new consumption trends should allow more

women’s sports coverage (Kian & Hardin, 2009; Pegoraro et al., 2021). Our results should

persuade sports consumers and managers to be mindful of biases when evaluating men’s and

women’s sports and making decisions about their promotion. Of course, consumers cannot

suddenly change the way they perceive sports, and gender bias will not disappear overnight

(Burton, 2015; LaVoi & Dutove, 2012; Micelotta et al., 2018). Nevertheless, the lack of

differences in perceived quality and the willingness to pay to watch a match are instructive.

                                                14
Disclosure statement
The authors report there are no competing interests to declare.

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study is publicly available in HarvardDataVerse,

https://doi.org/10.7910/DVN/P12G8I (only accessible upon publication). We deleted individual

identifiable information.

Funding details

The author(s) received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this

article.

Acknowledgments

For their comments and suggestions, we would like to thank conference participants at the HSE

Analytics for Management and Economics Conference (October, 2020), the 96th WEAI Annual

Conference (June 27‐July 1, 2021), the XI CIED Conference (June 2‐4, 2021), the WEAI

International Conference (March 17‐19, 2021), and seminar participants at the NTNU Spring

Seminar, Center for Sport and Culture Management Research (March, 2021).

References

Arechar, A. A., Gächter, S., & Molleman, L. (2018). Conducting interactive experiments online.

           Experimental Economics, 21(1), 99–131. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-017-9527-2

                                                15
Åslund, O., & Skans, O. N. (2012). Do anonymous job application procedures level the playing

       field? Industrial and Labor Relations Review, 65(1), 82–107.

       https://doi.org/10.1177/001979391206500105

Boring, A. (2017). Gender biases in student evaluations of teaching. Journal of Public

       Economics, 145, 27–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2016.11.006

Budden, A. E., Tregenza, T., Aarssen, L. W., Koricheva, J., Leimu, R., & Lortie, C. J. (2008).

       Double-blind review favours increased representation of female authors. Trends in

       Ecology and Evolution, 23(1), 4–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2007.07.008

Burch, L., Billings, A., & Zimmerman, M. (2018). Comparing American soccer dialogues: Social

       media commentary surrounding the 2014 US men’s and 2015 US women’s world Cup

       teams. Sport in Society, 21(7), 1047–1062.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2017.1284811

Burhan, A. (2022, April 2022). Barcelona Femení set another women’s world record attendance

       at Camp Nou. Forbes. Available at:

       https://www.forbes.com/sites/asifburhan/2022/04/22/barcelona-femen-set-another-

       womens-world-record-attendance-at-camp-nou/?sh=5b6a87694fc4Forbes

Burton, L. J. (2015). Underrepresentation of women in sport leadership: A review of research.

       Sport Management Review, 18(2), 155–165. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2014.02.004

Burton, L. J., Borland, J., & Mazerolle, S. M. (2012). “They cannot seem to get past the gender

       issue”: Experiences of young female athletic trainers in NCAA Division I intercollegiate

       athletics. Sport Management Review, 15(3), 304-317.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2012.01.001

                                               16
Cavalier, E., & Newhall, K. (2018). “Stick to soccer”: Fan reaction and inclusion rhetoric on

       social media. Sport in Society, 21(7), 1078–1095.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2017.1329824

Cheuvront, S. N., Carter, R., DeRuisseau, K. C., & Moffatt, R. J. (2005). Running performance

       differences between men and women. Sports Medicine, 35(12), 1017-1024.

       https://doi.org/10.2165/00007256-200535120-00002

Connell, R. W., & Messerschmidt, J. W. (2005). Hegemonic masculinity: Rethinking the concept.

       Gender and Society, 19(6), 829–859. https://doi.org/10.1177/0891243205278639

Cooky, C., Messner, M. A., & Hextrum, R. H. (2013). Women play sport, but not on TV.

       Communication & Sport, 1(3), 203–230. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167479513476947

Crandall, C. S., & Eshleman, A. (2003). A justification-suppression model of the expression and

       experience of prejudice. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 414–446.

       https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.414

Cunningham, G. (2013). Theory and theory development in sport management. Sport

       Management Review, 16(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2012.01.006

Darvin, L., Holden, J., Wells, J., & Baker, T. (2021). Breaking the glass monitor: examining the

       underrepresentation of women in esports environments. Sport Management Review,

       24(3), 475-499. https://doi.org/10.1080/14413523.2021.1891746

Devine, P. G., Forscher, P. S., Cox, W. T. L., Kaatz, A., Sheridan, J., & Carnes, M. (2017). A

       gender bias habit-breaking intervention led to increased hiring of female faculty in STEM

       departments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73(August), 211–215.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.07.002

                                                17
Dolan, K., & Sanbonmatsu, K. (2009). Balance in government. American Politics Research,

       37(3), 409–428. https://doi.org/10.1177/1532673X08322109

Ellemers, N. (2018). Gender stereotypes. Annual Review of Psychology, 69, 275–298.

       https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216- 011719

English, C. (2017). Toward sport reform: Hegemonic masculinity and reconceptualizing

       competition. Journal of the Philosophy of Sport, 44(2), 183-198.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/00948705.2017.1300538

Fink, J. (2013). Theory development in sport management: My experience and other

       considerations. Sport Management Review, 16(1), 17–21.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2011.12.005

Fink, J. S. (2015). Female athletes, women’s sport, and the sport media commercial complex:

       Have we really “come a long way, baby”? Sport Management Review, 18(3), 331-342.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2014.05.001

Funk, D. C., Ridinger, L. L., & Moorman, A. M. (2003). Understanding consumer support:

       Extending the Sport Interest Inventory (SII) to examine individual differences among

       women’s professional sport consumers. Sport Management Review, 6(1), 1–31.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/S1441-3523(03)70051-5

Gao, M. (2020, December 13). Women’s soccer set viewership records in 2020 — now it needs

       to keep them watching. CNBC. Available at: https://www.cnbc.com/2020/12/13/womens-

       soccer-viewership-records-paving-expansion.html

                                               18
García, J., & Rodríguez, P. (2002). The determinants of football match attendance revisited:

       Empirical evidence from the Spanish football league. Journal of Sports Economics, 3(1),

       18-38. https://doi.org/10.1177/152700250200300103

Gentzkow, M., & Shapiro, J. M. (2006). Media bias and reputation. Journal of Political Economy,

       114(2), 280–316. https://doi.org/10.1086/499414

Gisladottir, T., & Reid, P. A. (2021). Gender imprinting. In P. M. Pedersen (Ed.), Encyclopedia

       of Sport Management (pp. 210-211). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Goldin, C., & Rouse, C. (2000). Orchestrating impartiality: The impact of “blind” auditions on

       female musicians. American Economic Review, 90(4), 715–741.

       https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.90.4.715

Gomez-Gonzalez, C., Dietl, H., & Nesseler, C. (2019). Does performance justify the

       underrepresentation of women coaches? Evidence from professional women’s soccer.

       Sport Management Review, 22(5), 640–651. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2018.09.008

Gupta, V. K., & Turban, D. B. (2012). Evaluation of new business ideas: Do gender stereotypes

       play a role? Journal of Managerial Issues, 24(2), 140–156.

       https://www.jstor.org/stable/43488130

Heilman, M. E. (2001). Description and prescription how gender stereotypes prevent women

       ascent up. Journal of Social Issues, 57(4), 657–674. https://doi.org/10.1111/0022-

       4537.00234

Hindman, L. C., & Walker, N. A. (2020). Sexism in professional sports: How women managers

       experience and survive sport organizational culture. Journal of Sport Management, 34(1),

       64-76. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsm.2018-0331

                                               19
Hinnerich, B. T., Höglin, E., & Johannesson, M. (2015). Discrimination against students with

       foreign backgrounds: Evidence from grading in Swedish public high schools. Education

       Economics, 23(6), 660–676. https://doi.org/10.1080/09645292.2014.899562

Hoeber, L., & Kerwin, S. (2013). Exploring the experiences of female sport fans: A collaborative

       self-ethnography. Sport Management Review, 16(3), 326-336.

       https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2012.12.002

Hyde, M. (2019, June 12). Pity the poor man who’s had the Women’s World Cup shoved down

       his throat. The Guardian. Available at:

       https://www.theguardian.com/football/blog/2019/jun/12/womens-world-cup-shoved-

       down-mens-throats

Johnson, S. K., & Kirk, J. F. (2020). Dual-anonymization yields promising results for reducing

       gender bias: A naturalistic field experiment of applications for hubble space telescope

       time. Publications of the Astronomical Society of the Pacific, 132(1009), 34503.

       https://doi.org/10.1088/1538-3873/ab6ce0

Joncheray, H., Level, M., & Richard, R. (2016). Identity socialization and construction within the

       French national rugby union women’s team. International Review for the Sociology of

       Sport, 51(2), 162–177. https://doi.org/10.1177/1012690213517108

Kassouf, J. (2020, August 19). Sacramento closes in on the NWSL, again. The Equalizer.

       https://equalizersoccer.com/2020/08/19/sacramento-republic-nwsl-expansion-2021-report/

Kian, E. M., & Hardin, M. (2009). Framing of sports coverage based on the sex of sports writers:

       Female journalists counter the traditional gendering of media coverage. International

       Journal of Sport Communication, 2, 185–204. https://doi.org/10.1123/ijsc.2.2.185

                                                 20
Krause, A., Rinne, U., & Zimmermann, K. F. (2012). Anonymous job applications in Europe.

       IZA Journal of European Labor Studies, 1(1), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1186/2193-9012-1-

       5

Lawless, J. L. (2009). Sexism and gender bias in election 2008: A more complex path for women

       in politics. Politics and Gender, 5(1), 70–80.

       https://doi.org/10.1017/S1743923X09000051

Lee, W., & Cunningham, G. B. (2018). Group diversity’s influence on sport teams and

       organizations: a meta-analytic examination and identification of key moderators.

       European Sport Management Quarterly, 19(2), 139-159.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/16184742.2018.1478440

Leslie, S. J., Cimpian, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie

       gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347(6219), 262–265.

       https://doi.org/10.4135/9781483392240.n8

Litchfield, C., & Kavanagh, E. (2019). Twitter, team GB and the Australian Olympic team:

       Representations of gender in social media spaces. Sport in Society, 22(7), 1148–1164.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2018.1504775

Matheson, D. (2021, July 7). Bridge-building time for women’s soccer. CBC.

       https://www.cbc.ca/playersvoice/entry/diana-matheson-bridge-building-time-for-womens-

       soccer

Mendoza, C., & Zurka, K. (2021, December 17). The summit for democracy: Advancing gender

       equity and equality for representative societies. U.S. Department of State. Available at:

                                                21
https://www.state.gov/dipnote-u-s-department-of-state-official-blog/the-summit-for-

       democracy-advancing-gender-equity-and-equality-for-representative-societies/

Mengel, F., Sauermann, J., & Zölitz, U. (2019). Gender bias in teaching evaluations. Journal of

       the European Economic Association, 17(2), 535–566. https://doi.org/10.1093/jeea/jvx057

Micelotta, E., Washington, M., & Docekalova, I. (2018). Industry gender imprinting and new

       venture creation: The liabilities of women’s leagues in the sports industry.

       Entrepreneurship Theory and Practice, 42(1), 94–128.

       https://doi.org/10.1177/1042258717732778

Mitra, D., & Golder, P. N. (2006). How does objective quality affect perceived quality? Short-

       term effects, long-term effects, and asymmetries. Marketing Science, 25(3), 230-247.

       https://doi.org/10.1287/mksc.1050.0175

Morganson, V. J., Jones, M. P., & Major, D. A. (2010). Understanding women’s

       underrepresentation in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics: The role of

       social coping. Career Development Quarterly, 59(2), 169–179.

       https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-0045.2010.tb00060.x

Moss-Racusin, C. A., Dovidio, J. F., Brescoll, V. L., Graham, M. J., & Handelsman, J. (2012).

       Science faculty’s subtle gender biases favor male students. Proceedings of the National

       Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(41), 16474–16479.

       https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1211286109

Negley, C. (2019, December 16). Charlotte’s MLS expansion fee reportedly a record $325m --

       two-thirds more than recent expansions. Yahoo Sports. https://sports.yahoo.com/charlotte-

                                                22
will-reportedly-become-30th-mls-franchise-for-record-325-m-twothirds-more-than-

       recent-expansions-212710659.html

Pawlowski, T., Nalbantis, G., & Coates, D. (2018). Perceived game uncertainty, suspense and the

       demand for sport. Economic Inquiry, 56(1), 173-192. https://doi.org/10.1111/ecin.12462

Pegoraro, A., Comeau, G. S., & Frederick, E. L. (2018). # SheBelieves: The use of Instagram to

       frame the US Women’s Soccer Team during# FIFAWWC. Sport in Society, 21(7), 1063-

       1077. https://doi.org/10.1080/17430437.2017.1310198

Pegoraro, A., Kennedy, H., Agha, N., Brown, N., & Berri, D. (2021). An analysis of broadcasting

       media using social media engagement in the WNBA. Frontiers in Sports and Active

       Living, 3, 658293. https://doi.org/10.3389/fspor.2021.658293

Régner, I., Thinus-Blanc, C., Netter, A., Schmader, T., & Huguet, P. (2019). Committees with

       implicit biases promote fewer women when they do not believe gender bias exists. Nature

       Human Behaviour, 3(11), 1171–1179. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-019-0686-3

Sah, S., Robertson, C. T., & Baughman, S. B. (2015). Blinding prosecutors to defendants’ race: A

       policy proposal to reduce unconscious bias in the criminal justice system. Behavioral

       Science & Policy, 1(2), 69–76. https://doi.org/10.1353/bsp.2015.0013

Scheadler, T., & Wagstaff, A. (2018). Exposure to women’s sports: Changing attitudes toward

       female athletes. The Sport Journal, 20(11), 1–14.

Silverman, A. (2020, September 28). The sports industry’s Gen Z problem. Available at:

       https://morningconsult.com/2020/09/28/gen-z-poll-sports-fandom/

Snowberg, E., & Yariv, L. (2021). Testing the waters: Behavior across participant pools.

       American Economic Review, 111(2), 687–719. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20181065

                                               23
Sveinson, K., Hoeber, L., & Toffoletti, K. (2019). “If people are wearing pink stuff they’re

       probably not real fans”: Exploring women’s perceptions of sport fan clothing. Sport

       Management Review, 22(5), 736-747. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2018.12.003

Taylor, E. A., Ward, R. M., Huml, M. R., & Darvin, L. (2022). Examining intercollegiate athletic

       department employees’ workplace behaviors using latent profile analysis. Sport

       Management Review, 25(1), 162-187. https://doi.org/10.1080/14413523.2021.1899720

Taylor, T., Fujak, H., Hanlon, C., & O’Connor, D. (2020). A balancing act: Women players in a

       new semi-professional team sport league. European Sport Management Quarterly, 1-21.

       https://doi.org/10.1080/16184742.2020.1815821

Travers, A. (2008). The sport nexus and gender injustice. Studies in Social Justice, 2(1), 79–101.

       https://doi.org/10.26522/ssj.v2i1.969

Trolan, E. J. (2013). The impact of the media on gender inequality within sport. Procedia - Social

       and Behavioral Sciences, 91, 215–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.08.420

UNESCO. (2021). Gender equality in sports media. Available at:

       https://en.unesco.org/themes/gender-equality-sports-media

United Nations (2022). Empower women in the workplace, marketplace and community. United

       Nations Global Compact. Available at: https://www.unglobalcompact.org/what-is-gc/our-

       work/social/gender-equality

Valenti, M., Scelles, N., & Morrow, S. (2020). The determinants of stadium attendance in elite

       women’s football: Evidence from the UEFA Women’s Champions League. Sport

       Management Review, 23(3), 509–520. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smr.2019.04.005

Valian, V. (1999). Why so slow? The advancement of women. The MIT Press.

                                                24
Wicker, P., Cunningham, G. B., & Fields, D. (2019). Head coach changes in women’s college

       soccer: An investigation of women coaches through the lenses of gender stereotypes and

       the glass cliff. Sex Roles, 81(11), 797-807. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01022-2

Witteman, H. O., Hendricks, M., Straus, S., & Tannenbaum, C. (2019). Are gender gaps due to

       evaluations of the applicant or the science? A natural experiment at a national funding

       agency. The Lancet, 393(10171), 531–540. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-

       6736(18)32611-4

Zimbalist, A. (2019, April 10). Female athletes are undervalued, in both money and media terms.

       Forbes. Available at: https://www.forbes.com/sites/andrewzimbalist/2019/04/10/female-

       athletes-are-undervalued-in-both-money-and-media-terms/?sh=696e28be13ed

                                               25
Figure 1. Example of unblurred videos (L) and blurred videos (R)

Note: a. The left image represents the characteristics of the unblurred condition (the gender of the
players is clear to see). The right image represents the characteristics of the blurred condition (the
   gender of the players is concealed). b. These images are not extracted from the videos in the
       sample, which omit plays that involve corners, free kicks, penalty kicks, or tackles.

                                                 26
Figure 2. Evaluation for all videos under the two experimental conditions (average values)

 Note: The unblurred condition represents the original videos where the gender of the players is
clear to see (control group). The blurred condition represents the blurred videos where the gender
   of the players is concealed (treatment group). The outcome variable is the evaluation of the
                    overall performance of the players (1 = poor; 5 = excellent).

                                               27
Figure 3. Differences in evaluation between female and male soccer videos under the two
                            experimental conditions (average values)

Note: a. The unblurred condition represents the original videos where the gender of the players is
clear to see (control group). The blurred condition represents the blurred videos where the gender
  of the players is concealed (treatment group). b. The outcome variable is the evaluation of the
                    overall performance of the players (1 = poor; 5 = excellent).

                                               28
Table 1. Regression results. Responses to unblurred videos

                                      Dependent variable: Video evaluation
                                    Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Female video                        -0.17*** -0.17*** -0.17*** -0.17***
                                    (0.054)   (0.054)     (0.053)   (0.053)

Gender of participant (female)                   0.048      0.042        0.085
                                                (0.054)    (0.054)      (0.062)

Age of participant                               0.002      0.002        0.002
                                                (0.003)     (0.03)      (0.003)

Participant watches often                                    0.23*       0.22
                                                            (0.14)      (0.14)

Participant watches daily                                  -0.30***     -0.26***
                                                           (0.073)      (0.073)

Participant has no preference                              omitted      omitted

                                                            0.094         0.14
Participant prefers female soccer
                                                           (0.090)      (0.091)

Participant prefers male soccer                            -0.056       0.012
                                                           (0.11)       (0.11)

Sequence control                                                         Yes

Continent control                                                        Yes

Constant                             4.01***     3.92***      3.89***   3.87***
                                     (0.038)      (0.11)      (0.17)    (0.50)
Observations                            580        580          580       570
       2
Adj. R                                 0.016      0.017        0.049     0.090
Note: a. The outcome variable is the evaluation of the overall performance of
the players (1 = poor; 5 = excellent).
b. Standard errors in parentheses * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
c. Model 4 drops 10 observations from participants who did not report their
place of residence.

                                      29
Table 2. Regression results. Responses to blurred videos

                                      Dependent variable: Video evaluation
                                    Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Female video                         -0.027    -0.027      -0.023    -0.018
                                    (0.064)   (0.063)     (0.064)   (0.059)

Gender of participant (female)                  0.21***    0.19***    0.20***
                                                (0.064)    (0.065)    (0.066)

Age of participant                             0.013***    0.011***    0.024
                                               (0.003)     (0.003)    (0.021)

Participant watches often                                  -0.032       -0.25
                                                           (0.18)      (0.17)

Participant watches daily                                  -0.25***    0.023
                                                           (0.086)    (0.085)

Participant has no preference                              omitted    omitted

                                                             -0.10     -0.081
Participant prefers female soccer
                                                            (0.10)     (0.10)

                                                           -0.57***    -0.35**
Participant prefers male soccer
                                                            (0.13)     (0.14)

Sequence control                                                        Yes

Continent control                                                       Yes

Constant                             3.58***     3.00***      3.49***   3.13***
                                     (0.045)      (0.13)      (0.24)    (0.52)
Observations                            646        646          562       550
       2
Adj. R                                 0.000      0.041        0.102     0.214
Note: a. The outcome variable is the evaluation of the overall performance of
the players (1 = poor; 5 = excellent).
b. Standard errors in parentheses * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
c. Model 3 drops 84 observations due to a coding error for the question about
participants’ preference for male or female soccer. Model 4 drops some
additional observations from participants who did not report their place of
residence. Omitting these observations does not significantly change the
results in Models 1, 2 and 3.

                                      30
Appendix 1. Additional information and analyses

                             Table A1. Videos in the experiment

 Video              Match             Scoring Player    Length (in sec)   Female
    1       Tottenham – Barcelona       Ivan Rakitić              14       No
    2         Argentina - Croatia      Luka Modrić                6        No
    3         Liverpool - Bayern        Sadio Mané                14       No
    4         Manchester City -       Raheem Sterling             9        No
    5          Russia - Croatia       Denis Cheryshev             9        No
                  Donetsk
    6          Chelsea - Lyon          Erin Cuthbert              6        Yes
    7         Norway – England         Lucy Bronze                5        Yes
    8       Netherlands – Sweden      Jackie Groenen              7        Yes
    9      Nigeria – Korea Republic   Asisat Oshoala              13       Yes
   10      United States - Thailand    Alex Morgan                5        Yes

                                            31
Table A2. Allocation of female and male videos in subsequent screens (S) across sequences

                                           (Seq.)

           S1      S2      S3         S4     S5       S6      S7       S8      S9        S10
Seq. 1     ♂        ♂      ♀          ♂      ♂        ♀       ♀        ♂       ♀          ♀
Seq. 2     ♀        ♀      ♀          ♀      ♂        ♂       ♂        ♂       ♂          ♀
Seq. 3     ♀        ♂      ♀          ♂      ♀        ♂       ♀        ♂       ♀          ♂
Seq. 4     ♀        ♂      ♀          ♂      ♂        ♂       ♀        ♂       ♀          ♀
Seq. 5     ♀        ♂      ♀          ♀      ♂        ♂       ♂        ♀       ♀          ♂
Seq. 6     ♀        ♀      ♀          ♀      ♂        ♂       ♂        ♂       ♂          ♀
Seq. 7     ♂        ♀      ♀          ♀      ♂        ♀       ♂        ♂       ♀          ♂
Notes: ♀ female video; ♂ male video

                                             32
Table A3. Regression results. Responses to unblurred videos. Interaction terms

                                       Dependent variable: Video evaluation
                                     Model 1 Model 2      Model 3     Model 4
Male video # Male participant        omitted  omitted      omitted    omitted

Male video # Female participant      0.0059       -0.015      -0.000      0.048
                                     (0.077)     (0.077)     (0.076)     (0.082)

Female video # Male participant      -0.21***    -0.21***    -0.21***    -0.21***
                                     (0.076)     (0.076)     (0.075)     (0.074)

Female video # Female participant     -0.12       -0.14*      -0.13*      -0.087
                                     (0.077)     (0.077)     (0.076)     (0.082)

Age of participant                                0.002       0.002       0.001
                                                 (0.003)     (0.003)     (0.003)

Participant watches often                                      0.23*       0.22
                                                              (0.14)      (0.14)

Participant watches daily                                    -0.30***    -0.26***
                                                             (0.073)     (0.073)

Participant has no preference                    omitted     omitted     omitted

Participant prefers female soccer                 0.041       0.094        0.14
                                                 (0.090)     (0.090)     (0.091)

Participant prefers male soccer                   -0.15      -0.056       0.012
                                                 (0.11)      (0.11)       (0.11)

Sequence control                                                           Yes

Continent control                                                          Yes

Constant                               4.01***      3.96***       3.91*** 3.89***
                                       (0.054)      (0.139)      (0.172)  (0.50)
Observations                             580          580           580     570
       2
Adj. R                                  0.015        0.021         0.046   0.087
Note: a. The outcome variable is the evaluation of the overall performance of the
players (1 = poor; 5 = excellent).
b. Standard errors in parentheses * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
c. Model 4 drops 10 observations from participants who did not report their place
of residence.

                                        33
Table A4. Regression results. Responses to unblurred videos. Willingness to pay

                                      Dependent variable: Willingness to pay
                                     Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
 Female video                        -0.07**   -0.07**    -0.07**    -0.07**
                                     (0.029)   (0.029)    (0.027)    (0.027)

 Gender of participant (female)                   0.049*      0.048*     0.016
                                                 (0.029)     (0.027)    (0.032)

                                                                           -
 Age of participant                              -0.003**   -0.004**
                                                                        0.005***
                                                 (0.002)     (0.002)    (0.002)

 Participant watches often                                   0.16**     0.19***
                                                             (0.07)     (0.07)

 Participant watches daily                                   0.24***    0.22***
                                                             (0.036)    (0.037)

 Participant has no preference                              omitted     omitted

                                                              0.10**     0.085*
 Participant prefers female soccer
                                                             (0.045)    (0.046)

 Participant prefers male soccer                              0.11**     0.075
                                                             (0.054)    (0.057)

 Sequence control                                                         Yes

 Continent control                                                        Yes

 Constant                             0.79***     0.87***      0.47***   0.34***
                                      (0.020)     (0.059)     (0.085)    (0.25)
 Observations                           579         579          579       569
        2
 Adj. R                                0.009       0.021        0.158     0.178
 Note: a. The outcome variable is if a respondent would be willing to pay to
 see the whole match (0 or 1).
 b. Standard errors in parentheses * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
 c. Model 4 drops 10 observations from participants who did not report their
 place of residence.

                                       34
Table A5. Regression results. Responses to blurred videos. Willingness to pay

                                     Dependent variable: Willingness to pay
                                    Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4
Female video                         -0.006    -0.006     -0.001     -0.001
                                    (0.029)   (0.029)    (0.030)    (0.030)

Gender of participant (female)                  -0.027     -0.032     -0.042
                                               (0.030)    (0.031)    (0.033)

Age of participant                              0.000     0.003**     -0.012
                                               (0.002)    (0.002)    (0.011)

Participant watches often                                 0.25***    0.27***
                                                          (0.087)    (0.087)

Participant watches daily                                 0.15***    0.15***
                                                          (0.041)    (0.043)

Participant has no preference                             omitted    omitted

                                                           0.10**     0.13**
Participant prefers female soccer
                                                          (0.048)    (0.052)

                                                           0.064      0.091
Participant prefers male soccer
                                                          (0.062)    (0.070)

Sequence control                                                       Yes

Continent control                                                      Yes

Constant                             0.69***     0.70***        0.17     0.24
                                     (0.021)     (0.059)      (0.11)    (0.26)
Observations                           646         646          562      550
Adj. R2                               0.000       0.001        0.066    0.116
Note: a. The outcome variable is if a respondent would be willing to pay to
see the whole match (0 or 1).
b. Standard errors in parentheses * p < 0.10, ** p < 0.05, *** p < 0.01
c. Model 3 drops 84 observations due to a coding error for the question about
participants’ preference for male or female soccer. Model 4 drops some
additional observations from participants who did not report their place of
residence. Omitting these observations does not significantly change the
results in Models 1, 2, and 3.

                                      35
Appendix 2. Parameters of the manipulated videos in Adobe Premiere Pro

Mosaic function parameters:
   -   Feature details:
          o Stands and Fans (Surroundings):
                        • Mask feather: 10.0; Mask Opacity: 100.0 %; Mask Expansion: 0.0
                          – 10.0
                        • Horizontal Blocks: 10; Vertical Blocks: 10
                        • Sharp Colors: No
          o Players:
                        • Mask feather: 10.0; Mask Opacity: 100.0 %; Mask Expansion: 0.0
                          – 10.0
                        • Horizontal Blocks: 100; Vertical Blocks: 100
                        • Sharp Colors: Yes

Lumetri Color function parameters:
   -   Feature details:
          o “Black” Team:
          • HSL Secondary
          • Key
                  ▪ “Set Color” and “Add Color” with pipette in Video (select surroundings of
                       players); Invert Mask: Yes
                  ▪ Correction:
                  ▪

                                       ▪   Temperature: -100.0
                                       ▪   Tint: 100.0
                                       ▪   Contrast: 100.0
                                       ▪   Sharpen: 0.0
                                       ▪   Saturation: 0.0

          o “White” Team:
          • HSL Secondary
          • Key
               ▪ “Set Color” and “Add Color” with pipette in Video (select surroundings of
                  Players); Invert Mask: Yes

                                             36
▪   Correction:
▪

                  ▪   Temperature: 100.0
                  ▪   Tint: -100.0
                  ▪   Contrast: 100.0
                  ▪   Sharpen: 0.0
                  ▪   Saturation: 0.0

                        37
You can also read