DECONSTRUCTING THE DISCOURSE OF STRENGTHS IN FAMILY LITERACY
←
→
Page content transcription
If your browser does not render page correctly, please read the page content below
Journal of Reading Behavior 1995, Volume 27, Number 4 DECONSTRUCTING THE DISCOURSE OF STRENGTHS IN FAMILY LITERACY Elsa Auerbach University of Massachusetts at Boston The family literacy movement in the United States has come of age: since the publication of Denny Taylor's seminal book in 1983 (Taylor, 1983), countless schol- arly articles, research studies, and media reports on family literacy have appeared. In the past 12 months alone, three major edited volumes have been devoted exclusively to this topic (Holt, 1994; Weinstein-Shr & Quintero, 1995; Morrow, 1995). The Na- tional Center for Family Literacy (NCFL) estimates that there are now over 1,000 family literacy programs across the United States and argues that it is the best long- term solution to America's poverty problem, better even than school reform for "tack- ling undereducation and all the related social and economic problems" (Darling, 1992, p. 1). According to representatives of the U.S. Department of Education, Even Start, the largest family literacy initiative in the United States is not only an important part of the U.S. education agenda for the 1990s "but perhaps the key to reaching U.S. educational goals" (McKee & Rhett, 1995, p. 166). Other English-speaking countries, influenced by the U.S. movement, have begun family literacy initiatives of their own (RaPAL Bulletin, 1994; Harrison, 1995, Fine Print, 1994; Morrow &Paratore, 1993). Not only has family literacy come to be seen as a state of the art approach to educa- tional reform, but, according to Street (1995), it can be said to have gained the status of a "literacy campaign." As such, I think we have come to a point where we need to do some stock-taking—to look at where we have been and where we want to go. Shortly after the appearance of Taylor's book, the first family literacy programs were established, many of which focused on transmitting school literacy practices into the home. At that time, I raised concerns about the dangers of a deficit perspec- tive on family literacy (Auerbach, 1989). In the past 5 years, I think a second genera- tion of family literacy programs has emerged, one in which virtually all of the proponents of family literacy claim to oppose deficit perspectives and to embrace 643 Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
644 Journal of Reading Behavior family strengths. In fact, adopting an antideficit stance has come to be seen as a sine qua non for gaining legitimacy within the field. The discourse of family literacy is permeated with calls for cultural sensitivity, celebration of diversity, and empower- ment of parents. Quite simply, the rhetoric of deficit has been replaced with a rhetoric of "strengths." It is this new generation of family literacy programs and approaches that I want to focus on in this article. My sense is that the very success of the family literacy movement and its widespread adoption of the discourse of strengths contains new dangers. I hope to show that the antideficit rhetoric has become so pervasive that it masks fundamental underlying differences in values, goals, ideological orientations, and pedagogical approaches. Despite apparent agreement on the need to combat deficit frameworks, the postdeficit generation of family literacy approaches is by no means monolithic. In fact, I will argue that, disclaimers notwithstanding, a significant tendency within the current generation of family literacy approaches may, in fact, represent a neodeficit ideology and that the discourse of strengths may, wittingly or unwittingly, serve the function of legitimating that ideology. An important task as family literacy gains ascendancy within the educational reform movement is to deconstruct this discourse and to get beyond surface dichotomies. Thus, the purpose of this paper is to take a critical look at various tendencies within the current generation of approaches—at what characterizes each, and what differentiates them from each other. I will broadly group these tendencies into three categories: the intervention prevention approach to family literacy, the multiple- literacies approach, and the social change approach. In doing so, my intention is not to present solutions or prescriptions, but rather to ask questions and to compli- cate the discussion. THE INTERVENTION PREVENTION APPROACH The intervention prevention model for family literacy posits that America's lit- eracy problems are rooted in undereducated parents' inability to promote positive literacy attitudes and interactions in the home. Since parents are seen as children's first teachers, they are said to bear primary responsibility for children's literacy de- velopment. According to this view, when parents themselves do not adequately use or value literacy, they perpetuate a cycle of undereducation which is at the root of America's social and economic problems. Since literacy problems are seen to origi- nate in families, the remedy must be located there; as one advocate puts it, "the seeds of school failure are planted in the home, and we cannot hope to uproot the problem by working only within the schools. We must approach it through the fam- ily" (Darling, 1992, p. 5). As such, proponents of this view support intervention programs aimed at changing parents' beliefs about literacy and literacy interactions with their children (Nickse, Speicher, & Buchek, 1988). Such programs are seen as the Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 645 best means to ensure that patterns of undereducation and illiteracy will be prevented from passing from generation to generation. The objectives of intervention are framed in terms of "[breaking] the intergenerational cycle of under-education and poverty, one family at a time, by changing the 'messages' communicated in the home" (NCFL, 1994, p. 3). This model is said to hold promise not only for individual families, but for the nation as a whole, in terms of addressing pervasive economic and educational problems which get in the way of meeting the challenges of a global economy. Proponents of the intervention prevention model often explicitly oppose deficit perspectives on family literacy and embrace so-called "wealth" or "strengths" mod- els of family literacy. They posit that all learners possess strengths and prior knowl- edge which can become the basis for learning; they emphasize building on the wealth of resources that participants already possess, arguing, for example, that where "a deficit model reinforces [parents'] fears, a strengths model honors their capabilities" (Potts, 1992, p. 3). Advocates of the strengths model often use terms such as critical thinking, problem-solving, student inquiry, cooperative learning, culturally rel- evant lessons, a whole language orientation, and student participation. Quality family literacy programs are said to promote "critical and creative thinking" and "build upon strengths, empower families... incorporate goal setting facilitate active learning..., utilize whole language strategies, and celebrate diversity" (NCFL, 1994, p. 4). Despite claims to the contrary, I would argue that the intervention prevention paradigm rests on a deficit perspective both in terms of its analysis of "the problem" and in terms of its proposed "solutions." Classic deficit views blame marginalized people for their own marginalization (locating the source of their problems with ge- netic, cultural, or linguistic deficiencies); inherent inadequacies are said to account for the underachievement and exclusion of particular populations from the various domains of mainstream society. The intervention prevention paradigm shifts the burden from genetic or linguistic factors to social or educational factors, locating the source of literacy, economic, and educational problems with deficiencies in family literacy practices and attitudes. Children's skills deficiencies are attributed to the inadequacies of parents' skills and beliefs about literacy; blanket claims such as the following continue to permeate the family literacy literature: "millions of parents with poor reading skills cannot engage in [reading to their children] because of their own reading deficiencies, and millions of others have neither the knowledge of its impor- tance nor the skills to read to their children" with the result that there is an ongoing "lack of parental reading models, in-school reading problems, and poor attitudes toward reading and education in general" (Nickse et al., 1988, p. 635). Advocates of intervention often paint bleak pictures of the home life of children of poor, undereducated families with statements like the following: "No one at home would read books, newspapers, or magazines. There were no library visits or books given as presents. No one even checked on whether the children had done their homework for school I discovered an intergenerational disease—parents who passed illiteracy Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
646 Journal of Reading Behavior and poverty along to their children" (Mansbach, 1993, p. 37). Others say, "excellence in public school education is an empty dream for youth who go home each afternoon to families where literacy is neither practiced nor valued" and, where "educational attainment is actively discouraged by family and friends" (Darling, 1992, p. 1, 3). Further, these home conditions and parental attitudes are said to account not just for children's skills deficits, but for poverty itself. Because teenage mothers, for example, are said to be "trapped in the same environment which limited their childhood achieve- ments, most of these young women never manage to pull themselves out of poverty" with the result that "A family heritage of undereducation often perpetuates a cycle of unemployment or under-employment" (Darling, 1992, p. 3). The discourse surrounding this perspective is permeated with metaphors that suggest pathology; the most common is one of disease, with statements like, "We can cure the disease of illiteracy, but only if we dispense large doses of family lit- eracy" (Mansbach, 1993, p. 3). Other commonly used terms like "uproot," "trapped," and "break the cycle" suggest images of weeds and prisons; terms like "at risk" and "disadvantaged" invoke stereotypes of cultural deprivation. Clearly this discourse stands in stark contrast to that invoked by a "wealth" model (despite the fact that, in some cases, both discourses emanate from the same organizations). It is particularly noteworthy that these claims persist into the 1990s even though in the last decade there has been significant research challenging the generalization that the homes of poor people are literacy impoverished. Five years ago, I summarized a good deal of this research indicating that (a) there is enormous diversity in home literacy practices among and within cultural groups; (b) rather than being literacy impoverished, the home environments of poor, undereducated and language minority children often are rich with literacy practices and artifacts; (c) although beliefs about literacy and its payoffs vary, marginalized families generally not only value literacy, but see it as the single most powerful hope for their children; (d) even parents who themselves have limited literacy proficiency support their children's literacy acquisition in many ways (Auerbach, 1989). Without belaboring the point, I would like to mention several recent studies which further challenge claims that poor, undereducated and language minority families fail to support their children's literacy development. The first set of studies indicates that often low-income or low-literate parents are highly supportive of their children's literacy acquisition. Reviewing the literature about home environments conducive to literacy acquisition, Fitzgerald, Spiegel, and Cunningham contend that "there is as much (or more) variation in home literacy patterns within selected socioeconomic levels and/or cultural/ethnic groups as among them" (1991, p. 192). They found that both low- and high-literacy-level parents viewed literacy artifacts and events, in particular, interacting with books, as important in the preschool years; the primary difference between low- and high-literacy-level parents was that the low-literacy-level parents "tend to value the importance of early literacy artifacts and events even more [italics in original] than parents with higher literacy Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 647 levels" (1991, p. 208). Likewise, an ethnographic study comparing literacy events in low- and middle-income families found that both groups promoted a wide range of activities and experiences (Baker, Serpell, & Sonnenschein, 1995). Interestingly, this study found that "low-income parents reported more frequent literacy activities un- dertaken for the purpose of learning literacy than did middle-income parents" (Baker etal.,1995,p.l2). Additional research suggests that even parents who are themselves not literate in English can and do support their children's literacy acquisition in multiple ways. A study of the literacy activities and values of Mexican Americans (Ortiz, 1992) found that parents were not only very concerned with their children's academic achieve- ment, but spent a great deal of time reading and writing with their children. Goldenberg and Gallimore (1991), in examining the relative importance of school versus home factors in the literacy acquisition of Spanish-speaking children, concluded that, de- spite the school staff's assumptions about a lack of home academic support, in reality, parents were eager to help their children succeed in school and did so in numerous ways. Caplan, Choy, and Whitmore's (1992) study of Indochinese refugee families found that the parents' lack of English proficiency had little effect on their children's school performance, and, interestingly, that parental support for cultural maintenance seemed to enhance academic achievement. They concluded, "Rather than adopting American ways and assimilating into the melting pot, the most suc- cessful Indochinese families appear to retain their own traditions and values" (Caplan et al., 1992, p. 41). Taken together, these studies suggest that blanket assertions about a disregard for the value of literacy or an absence of literacy practices in poor homes ignore significant research evidence. A second aspect of the intervention prevention model that I would like to problematize relates to proposed solutions or program objectives—the content of family literacy programming. When "the problem" is framed in terms of inadequate family literacy practices, beliefs and values, the remedy is framed in terms of chang- ing family behaviors and attitudes within families. In some cases, program objec- tives focus on changing behaviors and attitudes related to specific literacy practices (most commonly story-reading), and in others, they focus on altering the broad patterns of family interaction in which literacy is embedded. One of the most common intervention prevention models is what I would call the "bullet" program model in which programs offer single practice solutions like training parents to read stories or sending books home in backpacks; in their most extreme form, these single practice models suggest that schooling problems would be solved if only parents read to their children everyday. Many are premised on the notion that it is necessary to find ways of extending school reading experiences into the home and teaching parents to support classroom instruction. The most common program objective of these "bullet" programs is teaching parents the value of story-reading and the behaviors associated with it. Such pro- grams are often based on the premise that parents themselves were reared in homes Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
648 Journal of Reading Behavior where positive experiences with print were sparse, and thus need to be taught the value of reading to their own children; often participants are taught that the single most important thing they can do to help a child succeed in school is read stories to them (e.g., Enz & Searfoss, 1995). The focus on teaching parents the value of reading stories to their children, however, must itself be problematized. No one would argue that story-reading is unimportant; yet, numerous studies bring into question the view that story reading is the single best practice because it is based on flawed assumptions. First, giving instruction in the value of story-reading assumes that low-income parents don't already feel that it is important, an assumption which may be unfounded according to several of the studies I have already mentioned (e.g., Baker et al., 1995; Goldenberg & Gallimore, 1991). Second, the focus on story-reading may promote one kind of literacy event at the expense of others, thus undermining the integration of a range of literacy activities in ongoing family life and ignoring the value of other positive culture-specific practices (like storytelling or reading from religious texts). The nurturing of these cultural traditions, in fact, may be a critical basis, not only for enhancing cultural identity, but for supporting academic achieve- ment (Ferdman, 1990; Caplan et al., 1992). As Baker et al. say, "Although storybook reading is widely regarded by educators as an important means by which parents prepare their children for school, an absence of storybook reading does not neces- sarily mean that children are growing up without exposure to literacy practices" (p. 2). Further, the single practice solution directly contradicts a key finding of family literacy research—that it is a range of literacy practices integrated in a meaningful way into the fabric of daily life that promotes literacy acquisition (Taylor, 1983). Baker et al. argue that, "One of the recurrent themes of recent crosscultural research is that there are many different routes to successful developmental outcomes. Teachers may be more helpful to parents by suggesting multiple resources and techniques than by seeking to define a single, ideal pattern of parenting" (1995, p. 14). Finally, missing from this model is any notion of how the children's home experiences might inform classroom instruction. Other programs focus more broadly on patterns of interaction within families, aiming to change the parenting practices in which literacy is embedded. Often these programs are framed in terms of family strengths; they emphasize identifying existing "healthy" family traits, acknowledging the culture-specificity of norms for family strengths, and involving participants in setting their own goals (Potts, 1992; Potts & Paull, 1995). I certainly see each of these as important principles in their own right and significant steps forward from programs which only emphasize family inadequa- cies. Even these programs, however, may unwittingly fall into the trap of ignoring research about cultural variability in discourse styles and literacy practices, promot- ing culture-specific norms and values, and decontextualizing family life, thus ulti- mately putting responsibility for social problems on family shoulders. First, the particular selection of "healthy traits" and programmatic suggestions for enhancing or augmenting them may be culture specific. One formulation, for Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 649 example, provides a "menu" of universal traits from which to choose in curriculum development and goes on to identify good communication as the single most critical trait (Potts, 1992, p. 6). Some characteristics of "healthy family communication" in this model include: "[Family members] listen attentively and actively to what other[s] say, possibly summarizing the message, rephrasing it, or asking for clarification; they write notes to those in the household and letters to those in other places; they own and express feelings, both positive and negative" (p. 8). Characteristics of the trait "time together" include playing games as well as watching TV together. Suggestions for enhancing the "time together" trait include basing lessons on McDonald's menus, Little League brochures, scouting manuals, TV guides (Potts, 1992, p. 11). My concern is that such characteristics do not take into account the cultural and contextual variability of literacy practices and discourse styles which has been so extensively documented in ethnographic research (e.g., Heath, 1983; Reder, 1987). For example, expressing feelings openly may be entirely inappropriate in some cul- tures; writing notes to household members presupposes particular literacy practices and proficiency; summarizing and rephrasing messages is one narrowly culture- specific discourse style. Likewise, playing games across generations may be neither culturally familiar nor feasible for families (e.g., if parents work two jobs); watching TV together assumes that families can afford a TV and have time to watch together. The cultural specificity of the artifacts chosen as lesson materials requires little comment. Further, goals or positive outcomes are often framed in terms of attaining mainstream norms such as higher scores on standardized tests, viewing education "differently," attending more school functions, and understanding teachers' and administrators' problems better. My overall point here is that, despite aiming to be culturally sensitive and descriptive, the traits and characteristics, as they are pre- sented, may in fact be culture-specific and prescriptive, leading toward conformity to particular values and expectations. Further, for many programs, the process of identifying strengths seems to be a pretext for intervening in the internal workings of family life to change behaviors and values; programs often include instruction in "parenting skills" and behavior man- agement techniques. Again we need to problematize the notions of changing values and teaching parenting skills: Who gets to decide what values are adequate and what "good parenting" entails? Are there universals of good parenting? Do middle class academic "experts" know better than low-income African-American parents, or Cambodian refugee parents how they should raise their children to deal with the challenges of economic survival, racism, or cultural transition? I would argue that any program which aims to change values, beliefs, messages or behaviors raises significant ethical questions; as Shanahan and Rodriguez-Brown say, "Family lit- eracy programs raise... ethical problems because of their attempt to change parent values with regard to their children's education, and parent-child relations such as the use and sharing of literacy among parents and children" (1994, p. 2). Minimally, we need to proceed with caution and humility (rather than claiming to know what is best for others). Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
650 Journal of Reading Behavior A final aspect of the intervention prevention model that I want to consider here is the claim that family literacy offers a solution to both personal and national eco- nomic problems; it is often argued that family literacy will enable both individual families to find their way out of poverty as well as enabling the country as a whole to meet the challenges of the next century. The argument that literacy training will, in itself, lead to employment disregards macro economic factors like recession and unemployment patterns, social factors like job discrimination, as well as the actual dynamics of hiring and job retention. Census statistics, for example, indicate that race and gender override education as determinants of income and job status: white males with high school degrees have higher mean incomes than African-American males with college degrees or than women of any race with graduate degrees; even with gains in education over the past decade, African-Americans' overall poverty levels have increased (Boston Globe, 1995). The danger of this aspect of the inter- vention stance is that it serves the ideological function of scapegoating marginalized people for problems that originate in the broader socioeconomic structure. As Aus- tralian literacy theorist Alan Luke (1992-93) says, focusing on literacy as the curative for an ailing economy ignores significant macroeconomic factors: "In a global divi- sion of labor where transnational corporations far outstrip many national govern- ments in influence and power, economic growth and decline are influenced by a range of economic, political and social factors (e.g., labour costs; environmental law; government/industry relations; multinational investment,...) of which worker skill level is but one" (p. 20). On a micro level, the work of Hull (1991) illustrates the problems with arguing that literacy is the key determinant of employment opportunity. Her ethnographic study of a group of African-American women participating in a training program in order to get off welfare found that once the women got jobs, factors completely unrelated to this training determined who did and did not keep their jobs. Low wages, hours that conflicted with family responsibilities, intolerable working conditions, and demeaning treatment by their bosses eventually forced many of the women out. Broad contextual factors shaped by employer practices had more to do with employ- ment possibilities than did individual employees' literacy proficiency. Thus, for me, the most disturbing problem with the intervention prevention model is that it justifies putting responsibility for societal problems on family shoul- ders, implying that social change must be rooted in family change. The focus on the unit of the family as the locus of change excludes consideration of social, economic, or institutional forces which may constrain family life and impede literacy develop- ment. Even the notion of empowerment is framed in individual terms: power means the ability to transform one's own life through individual effort based on self-esteem; a sense of self-worth will lead to a sense of responsibility which in turn will lead to making a better life for one's family—a psychological version of pulling oneself up by the bootstraps. Taken to its logical extreme, this argument suggests that if parents change the messages that they send to children, the problems of education, poverty, Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 651 unemployment, crime, drug abuse, and teen pregnancy will be solved. The flip side of this argument is that social problems can be attributed to the failures of the family; given the demographics and patterns of child-rearing, this analysis, in its most sim- plistic version, could be said to imply that, once again, mothers are to blame (this time for the problems of the nation)! Obviously, no one would actually claim these absurdly reductionist statements as their position; yet my fear is that they represent an ideological tendency implicit in the intervention model. Thus, my concern is that the current intervention prevention perspective shares an underlying rationale with the classic deficit model for family literacy (Street, 1995) with the key difference that it explicitly attacks deficit perspectives using the dis- course of strengths. My own feeling is that within a political climate all too ready to blame families for social problems, couching an intervention prevention model in a rhetoric of strengths is particularly problematic. Precisely because deficit perspec- tives have been widely discredited, an overt antideficit stance may serve a rational- izing function, masking underlying deficit views with an aura of credibility. If it has this effect, regardless of the intention, the intervention rationale may be dangerous not despite the strengths rhetoric, but rather because of it. THE MULTIPLE-LITERACIES PERSPECTIVE A second perspective, what I am calling the multiple-literacies perspective, uses terminology which, at times, is similar to that of the intervention prevention model (e.g., wealth, cultural sensitivity, whole language, empowerment), but is based on a different set of assumptions and goals. Where the intervention model defines the problem as flawed home literacy practices and the solution as changing patterns of family interaction, the multiple-literacies perspective defines the problem as a mis- match between culturally variable home literacy practices and school literacies; it sees the solution as investigating and validating students' multiple literacies and cultural resources in order to inform schooling (Moll, 1992; Street, 1995). Where the intervention model advocates cultural sensitivity as a respectful stance, the mul- tiple-literacies model sees an understanding of cultural practices as the centerpiece of curriculum development. Where the intervention model advocates individual em- powerment through self-esteem and personal responsibility, the multiple-literacies perspective promotes empowerment through affirmation of cultural identity and com- munity building. Much of the work that has been done to develop this model for family literacy is informed by ethnographic research and focuses on immigrant or refugee families. Rather than focusing on specific programs, my discussion of this perspective will focus on its principles and practices. The starting point of the multiple-literacies perspective is that, whatever their literacy proficiency, participants bring with them culture-specific literacy practices and ways of knowing; it posits that "people with literacy difficulties in some parts of Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
652 Journal of Reading Behavior their lives already have some knowledge of literacy and live in cultural settings where various kinds of literacy are valued" (Street, 1995, p. ii). Regardless of educa- tional background, the households of poor and language-minority families are rich with "funds of knowledge" which often are unrecognized and untapped by educa- tors (Moll, 1992). Thus, according to this perspective, the starting point for programming must be a stance of inquiry which recognizes that "our own ways of knowing are no longer the ultimate authority" (Weinstein-Shr & Quintero, 1995, p. 112). The task, then, is to listen to students, to find out about their lives and cultural contexts, and to make room for their literacy practices in teaching; the premise here is that "The best teach- ers are those who can listen and learn, not just impart what they know to others" (Street, 1995, p. iii). Weinstein-Shr (1995) promotes this stance of inquiry as the single most important aspect of a culturally sensitive model, urging teachers to learn about the educational resources that language-minority families bring with them as well as the sociolinguistic rules and parenting practices of their cultures. As teachers are repositioned as learners, parents and community members be- come the experts. There are a number of ways that programs implement this principle. In some cases, teachers are trained to investigate home and community literacy practices for the purpose of informing instruction; Moll (1992) and colleagues have developed a framework in which teachers research the households of their students in order to uncover the funds of knowledge which can then inform their own curricu- lum development. In other cases, community resources are brought directly into the classroom. In Madigan's (1995) work, for example, family and community members were invited in as "outside experts" to share the ways that they had used literacy in their lives to support social change. A third approach is to involve learners in the research process as coinvestigators of literacy practices, values and beliefs. Neuman and her colleagues, for example, set up peer discussion groups of the teen mothers in their program to investigate their beliefs about learning and early literacy, arguing that "a critical part of the empowerment process... may be to learn from the parents themselves, their beliefs, values and practices within their home and community" (Neuman, Hagedorn, Celano, & Daly, in press). Gadsden, likewise, reports that par- ents in Philadelphia's Parent-Child Learning Project (PCLP) explored literacy activi- ties, purposes, questions, and issues that arose in their families (1995). The multiple-literacies view posits that not only should programs be informed by participants' beliefs and practices, but they should incorporate culturally famil- iar and relevant content. In such programs, curriculum materials often include genres (e.g., folktales, fables) and stories from the home culture or language (Ada, 1988). Themes related to the home culture may also be incorporated; a project for Mexican American parents, for example, included units on plants (eg. herbal medicines) and cotton (e.g., harvesting, uses) which drew on their agricultural background (Huerta- Macfas, 1995). Curricula often focus on sharing stories, an approach in which partici- pants read, write and talk about their lived histories—their own childhood memories as well as their experiences, traumas and triumphs (Arrastia, 1995; McGrail, 1995). Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 653 A key principle for insuring the relevance and appropriateness of content is learner participation in the curriculum development process: involving learners in selecting curriculum goals and themes is a common practice in culturally sensitive programs. This kind of involvement can go beyond the selection of thematic units to fundamentally shaping the direction of programs. Programs which follow this ap- proach often find that parents do not want to focus exclusively on child-related matters, but rather want to develop literacy for their own purposes. Participants in a project in which I was involved focused on topics ranging from the political situation in Haiti to homelessness, workplace discrimination, and sexually transmitted dis- eases (Auerbach, 1994). The Hmong Literacy Project in Fresno, California found that parents' main reason for attending classes was to maintain their own history and culture for the sake of their children (Kang, Kuehn, & Herrell, forthcoming). The Hmong Literacy Project points to another key feature of this approach—the emphasis on cultural maintenance and negotiation rather than cultural assimila- tion. An important aspect of the Hmong project, for example, was preserving the first language by learning to read and write in it, as well as teaching it to children; likewise, the program focused on writing down oral histories before they were lost (Kang et al., forthcoming). Many programs emphasize critically examining cross-cultural parenting issues rather than training parents in parenting practices identified by "experts"; participants in Project LEIF explored the changes in power relations be- tween parents and children that often occur in immigrant families (Weinstein-Shr, 1995). In addition to familiar content, culturally familiar contexts for learning are cen- tral to this paradigm. One way of creating such contexts entails ensuring that the communities and the languages of the learners are.represented on the staff. Classes in the Chelsea project, for example, are taught by teams that include at least one member who is fluent in the students' first language. The primary aim of a project that I worked in (Auerbach, 1994) was to train people from the communities of the learners as literacy instructors (in this case for ESL, Haitian Creole, and Spanish literacy instruction). Some programs also incorporate culturally familiar pedagogical prac- tices even when they are incongruent with educators' own pedagogical understand- ings or preferences; for example, some classes in the project that I worked with opened with a prayer and included dictation exercises at the students' insistence. Clearly, when teachers come from the same cultural backgrounds as the learners, they are more able to selectively draw on familiar pedagogical practices. Another way to create this kind of a context is through the use and instruction of the first language. In some cases, parents are invited to choose whether to do writing activities in their first language or in English (McGrail, 1995; Paratore, 1995). In the Chelsea project, since the participants come from many language groups, all reading texts are in English, but parents break into small groups with others who share the same language for prereading discussions; they discuss key concepts in the first language, as well as connecting settings or experiences from the text to those of their own countries and cultures (Paratore, 1995). Weinstein-Shr gives the Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
654 Journal of Reading Behavior example of "parent circles" where participants explore issues of parenting with each other in their first language. Huerta-Macias (1995) reports that code-switching was an accepted and integral part of Project FIEL classes. Kang et al. (forthcoming) mention the strategy of opening every school-based meeting in the parents' first languages before any English is spoken as another way to make parents feel at home. Finally, many projects are now incorporating first-language literacy instruction as a basis for parents' transition to ESL (Auerbach, 1993; Spener, 1994), for children's literacy development and academic achievement (Cummins, 1981), for the enhance- ment of identity and self-esteem (Ferdman, 1990), as well as for cultural maintenance (Kang et al., forthcoming). THESOCIAL CHANGE PERSPECTIVE The final perspective that I want to discuss encompasses all of the principles of the multiple-literacies tendency, but goes beyond them, placing its emphasis on issues of power as well as culture. It too uses the discourse of strengths and empow- erment, but differs from the other perspectives in terms of its assumptions and goals. The central assumption of this social change perspective is that problems of marginalized people originate in a complex interaction of political, social, and eco- nomic factors in the broader society rather than in family inadequacies or differences between home and school cultures; it is the conditions created by institutional and structural forces which shape access to literacy acquisition. In this view, it is these aspects of the social context, rather than individuals which must be the focus of change, as the following analysis suggests: Programs which define literacy as a set of skills or as the ability to use skills within work, community, or cultural setting, face a danger of placing the whole burden for change on the individual adult learner. The people with limited skills become the focus of the needed change. A yet broader definition of literacy sees it in the context of social realities. Illiteracy, like other disadvantages such as unemployment, pov- erty and social discrimination, is also a result of social, political and economic structures that perpetuate inequality. According to this model, literacy is not just acquiring personal skills but also having access to knowledge and power to create change in the structures that keep people illiterate and made it difficult for them to achieve other human rights. (Gillespie, 1990, p. 18) For family literacy programs, this view means seeing children's literacy acquisition as shaped by many forces, only one of which is parental input; the Lutheran Settle- ment House Women's Program Family Literacy Curriculum summarizes this position as follows: While the Women's Program's approach to intergenerational literacy acknowledges the impact that parents have on their children's literacy skills, parents are not the sole reason for a child's success or failure. School systems, teachers, poverty, drugs, and institutional racism, sexism, and classism are all important factors which Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 655 effect a child's progress in school.... It is critical that family literacy programs recognize the many factors which effect a child's success or failure in school, rather than giving the sole responsibility or blame to the parents. (Gordon, 1991, p. i) This perspective may well correspond to many parents' own analyses, according to Gadsden (1995): parents in the PCLP pointed both to neighborhood conditions and peer pressure as barriers to their children's success, noting that "where a child lives matters in what happens to [his or her] literacy and education" (Gadsden, 1995, p. 297). She reports, "The parents valued the possibilities that literacy provided, but their images were tied to issues of discrimination that affected the quality of their neighborhood and their lives" (1995, p. 298). Thus, goals in a social change view focus more on changing the institutions and addressing the conditions which cause marginalization than on changing families. This approach is informed by the work of Paulo Freire (1981) and others who argue that literacy in itself does not lead to empowerment or solve economic problems; rather it must be linked to a critical understanding of the social context and action to change oppressive conditions. A key part of this analysis is that issues of power permeate every aspect of literacy acquisition; literacy education must take into ac- count power relationships within families (between parents and children, husbands and wives), within classrooms (between students and teachers), within programs (between participants and administrators), and within institutions (between parents and schools or government agencies). Change is seen to come about through a gradual process of struggling with inequities wherever they occur; the struggles in the more immediate domains (family, classroom) are both a part of and a rehearsal for struggles in the broader domains; the broader changes come about not just through individual effort but through collective action. Personal empowerment, thus, cannot be separated from social change: empowerment is defined not in individual terms (i.e., in terms of gaining self-esteem and taking control of one's life), but in social terms (in terms of challenging the institutional forces that impede access). Although cultural affirmation is an essential component of this process, it is not the primary goal. Not surprisingly, funding for programs that adhere to this model is sparse. McCaleb's Building communities of learners (1994) is one of the few books which presents an elaborated discussion of this perspective. Although many programs incorporate aspects of this model, few would say they are premised on it. For this reason, I will focus on its key principles and practices rather than on programs. The first principle relates to the issue of participant control. This aspect of the model is concerned with questions like: Who decides a program is necessary and gets it going? Who gets to name the issues, choose the themes, determine the goals? Who decides the research agenda and determines what counts as progress? Who speaks for the project? How are the teachers chosen? Programs often go beyond learner participation in setting individual goals or contributing to the selection of learning themes. In some cases, programs are initiated by parents themselves in response to needs that they have determined either in relation to themselves or in Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
656 Journal of Reading Behavior relation to their children's education. Plans for the Hmong project were initiated by parents who approached the principal of their children's elementary school (Kang et al., forthcoming). Delgado-Gaitan (1991) describes a project in California in which parents came together to form a group called COPLA (Comite de Padres Latinos) in order to pressure the schools to better meet the needs of their children. Some pro- grams consult community members in an advisory capacity in designing programs (or to do outreach). Likewise, participants may have decision-making roles in pro- gram administration; the Worker Family Education Program of the International La- dies' Garment Workers Union in New York, for example, has student councils which participate in curriculum, hiring, and administrative decisions. The notion of commu- nity control is certainly not uncomplicated: it is not always clear who the "commu- nity" is or what the affiliations of community representatives are (as Lewis & Varbero, 1995, show). Yet, as Gadsden says, study after study shows that "when participants are part of the decision-making process of a program that is intended 'to help them,' the program becomes more effective and the effect more durable" (1995, p. 7). A second aspect of the social change perspective is the notion of dialogue as a key pedagogical process. In place of skills training or the transfer of information from experts to learners, this model stresses an exchange among peers; participants share their experiences to gain a critical understanding of their social nature as well as to strategize for action. Going beyond cultural affirmation, dialogue becomes a vehicle for making sense of one's reality, which, in turn is the basis for transforming it. Delgado-Gaitain (1991) characterizes this critical reflection as, "a process that engages people in careful examination of the assumptions that guide self, family and institutional norms, values, and practices As a consequence, the group's aware- ness of their shared experience (past and present) becomes the basis for collective action" (1991, p. 34). In some cases, this dialogue takes the form of sharing experi- ences through storytelling (Arrastfa, 1995). In others, it takes the form of reading about critical social issues and discussing them (Gordon, 1991). McCaleb (1994) describes a project in which critical themes in community life were identified through dialogue among parents and were then used as the basis for books which were coauthored by parents and children. The social change perspective also incorporates content centering around criti- cal social issues from participants' lives. Where an intervention program might focus on parent-child interactions and a multiple literacies program on diverse cul- tural practices or literacy uses, a program concerned with social change would em- phasize the exploration of substantive problems that learners are encountering in their everyday lives. For example, a family literacy program in Los Angeles, which began the week after the "L.A. riots" used the classes to explore participants' fears and concerns; they then chose to write about their experiences and share them by publishing a book for local distribution QAqui Vivimos! Book Project, 1993). Like- wise, students in the projects described by Gadsden (1995) and McCaleb (1994) used reading and writing to explore important issues in their lives related to education, Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 657 neighborhoods, childrearing and schools. Madigan's (1995) work centered on invit- ing adults into classrooms to share their own experiences using reading and writing to achieve social change in their communities. In the Family Involvement Project in Washington, DC, parents wrote about topics like their jobs and immigration experi- ences and then shared their stories with their children and other parents (Rankin, 1993). Connected to the exploration of social issues as content for literacy curricula is the critical notion of action for social change. Once participants have an increased understanding of the social nature of problems they are confronting (e.g., that their children's problems in schools may be the result of institutional practices), they may work together to challenge institutions or change conditions which impede literacy acquisition. Actions can take many forms, from advocating for a particular kind of literacy program (as in the case of the Hmong project) to publishing a book for the wider community (as in the case of the L.A. project). A logical outcome of action- oriented family literacy programs is that they become parent involvement projects in the sense that participants become engaged in advocacy related to children's school- ing. Quintero (1995) describes an instance when several parents' concern about negative encounters with their children's school resulted in a family literacy lesson on advocacy; this, in turn, led to dialogue among parents who shared similar experi- ences. They then organized a broader meeting to develop strategies for dealing with "children's abuse by teachers" (Quintero, 1995, p. 154). The Right Question Project (Backman, 1993) in Boston focuses on training to enable low-income people to advo- cate for themselves and their children; their work centers on teaching parents to ask questions of experts, to assert their rights regarding their children's education, and ultimately to become involved in school reform efforts rather than leaving them in the hands of teachers or professionals. There is evidence to suggest that initiatives which take issues of power into account are, in fact, effective in engaging underrepresented parents in their own and their children's education. Delgado-Gaitan's (1991) study compared parental deci- sion-making roles in three programs, and found that even when program content was culturally relevant and meetings were conducted in the parents' home language, parental participation was limited if they were not involved in decision-making. On the other hand, participation and impact were significantly greater when parents organized themselves into an autonomous group, setting their own agenda, and making their own decisions. They learned from and provided support for each other, analyzing individual experiences to find commonalities, as well as identifying the knowledge they needed to take action; they then created an organization which pressured the district to train teachers to work with parents and to address problems in bilingual and ESL programs. Street (1990) extends this argument about the centrality of issues of power to the level of broad literacy campaigns (with obvious implicit messages for family literacy). He contends that what differentiates literacy campaigns that fail from those that Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
658 Journal of Reading Behavior succeed is whether or not they take power relationships into account: Literacy campaigns . . . always involve power relations: teachers, administrators, learners and politicians are disputing the right to define what is knowledge and truth and to spread their version of reality to the population at large.... The history of literacy campaigns in the recent past suggests that it is not always the seemingly more powerful outsiders who win these struggles. Indeed, it may be precisely because these deeper aspects of literacy practices have not been considered that many literacy campaigns have failed. (1990, p. 3) The above examples suggest that factors like cultural relevance, content related to critical issues, and dialogue are necessary, but not sufficient in themselves to constitute a social change perspective; rather, the key issue is the locus of control. What differentiates this perspective from others is the assumption that families and communities have the right to determine for themselves the direction of family lit- eracy and school involvement efforts, rather than assuming that outsiders know what is best for them. CONCLUSION I started this paper by saying that although the U.S. family literacy movement has explicitly rejected a deficit model, we need to look closely at the new generation of programs and policies that are gaining ascendancy. I have posited that there are several tendencies, all of which invoke the discourse of strengths and empowerment, but which have different rationales, objectives, and practices. On one extreme are those which seek to change families so they assimilate more successfully into the social order as it exists; on the other are those that seek to empower families to change those aspects of the social order that exclude them. I have tried to look beyond surface commonalities to identify features that differentiate these views, problematizing some of their claims and promises. Much of my paper has taken the form of a warning about the potential dangers accompanying the newfound suc- cesses of the family literacy movement. Having said this, I want to end by turning this warning back on myself. Just as the new rhetoric poses a threat, there is an equal threat in labeling perspectives. Although I have proposed conceptual categories, I see a real danger in glibly assigning any given program to one category or another. I hope that I have shown that each tendency has aspects of the others within it, and that programs often incorporate features of different tendencies; incorporating one of the practices that characterizes a particular perspective does not imply that a program should be labeled as adhering to that perspective. My intention is not to recreate dichotomies but rather to call for critical reflection on the direction of the family literacy movement as a whole. I would say that our challenge as researchers and practitioners (myself included) is to position ourselves as learners rather than experts, to take a stance of humility rather than authority, and to problematize solu- Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
Critical Issues 659 tions rather than prescribe them. My hope is that this analysis will further the debate about what family literacy can and cannot do, and what its legitimate role may be within a broader movement for educational reform and social change. REFERENCES !Aquí Vivimos! Voces de familias mexicanas y centroamericanas en el ciudad de Los Angeles. (1993). Los Angeles: !Aquí Vivimos! Book Project. Ada, A. (1988). The Pajaro Valley experience. In T. Skutnabb-Kangas & J. Cummins (Eds.), Minority education: From shame to struggle (pp. 223-238). Philadelphia, PA: Multilingual Matters. Arrastía, M. (1995). Our stories to transform them: A source of authentic literacy. In G. Weinstein- Shr & E. Quintero (Eds.), Immigrant learners and their families: Literacy to connect generations (pp. 101-109). McHenry, IL: Center for Applied Linguistics & Delta Systems. Auerbach, E. (1989). Toward a social-contextual approach to family literacy. Harvard Educa- tional Review, 59, 165-181. Auerbach, E. (1993). Re-examining English-Only in the ESL classroom. TESOL Quarterly, 27, 9-32. Auerbach, E. (1994). From the community—to the community: A guidebook for training com- munity literacy instructors. Boston: University of Massachusetts at Boston. Backman, S. (1993). Ask the right question. Rethinking Schools, 7(3), 13. Baker, L., Serpell, R., & Sonnenschein, S. (1995). Opportunities for literacy learning in the homes of urban preschoolers. In L. Morrow (Ed.), Family literacy connections in schools and communities (pp. 236-252). Newark, NJ: IRA. Caplan, N., Choy, M., & Whitmore, J. (1992). Indochinese refugee families and academic achieve- ment. Scientific American, 266(2), 36-42. Cummins, J. (1981). The role of primary language development in promoting educational suc- cess for language minority students. In California State Department of Education (Ed.), Schooling and language minority students: A theoretical framework (pp. 3-49). Los Ange- les: California State University. Darling, S. (1992). Family literacy: The need and the promise. Louisville, KY: National Center for Family Literacy. Delgado-Gaitan, C. (1991). Involving parents in the schools: A process of empowerment. American Journal of Education, 100, 20-46. Enz, B., & Searfoss, L. (1995). Let the circle be unbroken: Teens as literacy learners and teachers. In L. Morrow (Ed.), Family literacy connections in schools and communities (pp. 115-129). Newark, NJ: IRA. Ferdman, B. (1990). Literacy and cultural identity. Harvard Educational Review, 60, 181-204. Fine Print (1994). Special Issue on Family Literacy, 16(2), Victoria, Australia. Fitzgerald, J., Spiegel, D., & Cunningham, J. (1991). The relationship between parental literacy level and perceptions of emergent literacy. Journal of Reading Behavior, 23, 191-213. For U.S. blacks, losses and gains. (1995, February 22). Boston Globe, p. 6. Freire, P. (1981). Education for critical consciousness. New York: Continuum. Gadsden, V. (1995). Representations of literacy: Parents' images in two cultural communities. In L. Morrow (Ed.), Family literacy connections in schools and communities (pp. 287-303). Newark, NJ: IRA. Gillespie, M. (1990). Many literacies: Modules for training adult beginning readers and tutors. Amherst: Center for International Education, University of Massachusetts. Downloaded from jlr.sagepub.com by guest on March 7, 2015
You can also read